Value-based decision-making network functional connectivity correlates with substance use and delay discounting behaviour among young adults
Kavinash Loganathan
Jeggan Tiego
SimpleOriginal

Summary

Young adults show a continuum of substance use tied to impulsive decision-making. Stronger connectivity in value-based choice regions links to higher use and lower delay discounting, suggesting potential preaddiction markers.

2023

Value-based decision-making network functional connectivity correlates with substance use and delay discounting behaviour among young adults

Keywords Substance Use Disorders; Susceptibility Biomarkers; Functional Connectivity; Neuroimaging; Factor Mixture Modelling; Preaddiction; Delay Discounting; Brain Networks; Dimensional Models; Human Connectome Project

Abstract

Substance use disorders are characterized by reduced control over the quantity and frequency of psychoactive substance use and impairments in social and occupational functioning. They are associated with poor treatment compliance and high rates of relapse. Identification of neural susceptibility biomarkers that index risk for developing a substance use disorder can facilitate earlier identification and treatment. Here, we aimed to identify the neurobiological correlates of substance use frequency and severity amongst a sample of 1,200 (652 females) participants aged 22–37 years from the Human Connectome Project. Substance use behaviour across eight classes (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, sedatives, hallucinogens, cocaine, stimulants, opiates) was measured using the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism. We explored the latent organization of substance use behaviour using a combination of exploratory structural equation modelling, latent class analysis, and factor mixture modelling to reveal a unidimensional continuum of substance use behaviour. Participants could be rank ordered along a unitary severity spectrum encompassing frequency of use of all eight substance classes, with factor score estimates generated to represent each participant’s substance use severity. Factor score estimates and delay discounting scores were compared with functional connectivity in 650 participants with imaging data using the Network-based Statistic. This neuroimaging cohort excludes participants aged 31 and over. We identified brain regions and connections correlated with impulsive decision-making and poly-substance use, with the medial orbitofrontal, lateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices emerging as key hubs. Functional connectivity of these networks could serve as susceptibility biomarkers for substance use disorders, informing earlier identification and treatment.

1. Background

Substance use disorders (SUDs) describe a constellation of symptoms characterized by continuing use of one or more intoxicating substances despite significant negative consequences (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Symptoms include reduced control over the quantity and frequency of use, hazardous patterns of consumption, and accompanying impairments in social and occupational functioning (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Prevalence of SUDs is estimated as high as 12% for alcohol and 2–3% for illicit drugs (Merikangas & McClair, 2012). Additionally, SUDs are associated with significant social harms and often poor treatment response characterized by poor compliance and high rates of relapse (Miller, 1996). Thus, there is a need for earlier identification and treatment of SUDs (Yücel et al., 2019). For example, the term ‘preaddiction’ has been coined to refer to mild to moderate SUDs that have not yet progressed to severe levels (‘addiction’) and may represent a critical treatment window (McLellan et al., 2022). However, there are no objective biological assessments available for evaluating risk for SUD.

A central aim of contemporary psychiatric research is the identification of susceptibility biomarkers - measurable biological characteristics that index liability for psychiatric illness (Beauchaine, 2009, Califf, 2018, Singh and Rose, 2009). Susceptibility biomarkers hold great promise for improving mental health treatment through earlier identification of psychopathology (Cook, 2008). Over the past decade, there has been an interest in incorporating neurobiological findings into the diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders (Cuthbert and Insel, 2013, Hyman, 2007). Shared neurobiology emerged as a key validator introduced by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders – Fifth Edition (DSM-5) Task Force Study Group for exploring the proposed reorganization of diagnostic categories, including SUDs, into metastructures based on comorbidity, common etiology, course of illness, treatment response, and shared neural substrates (Andrews et al., 2009).

Research based on traditional psychiatric nosology has conspicuously failed to yield robust evidence of the neurobiological mechanisms underlying psychopathology (Hyman, 2007, Jablensky, 2016, Maj, 2014). To circumvent these limitations, psychiatric research is transitioning away from traditional categorizations of mental disorders as discrete diagnostic entities towards empirically-based, dimensional models of psychopathology, such as the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) (Cuthbert, 2014). Dimensional models are better positioned to identify shared aetiological mechanisms of psychiatric disorders by capturing phenotypic variation across the full spectrum of symptom severity (Cuthbert, 2014, Patrick et al., 2013). A wealth of evidence indicates that substance use problems constitute a dimensional continuum in the population with no natural demarcation point designating problematic from non-problematic use (Kraemer et al., 2004, Miettunen et al., 2016). There is also evidence to suggest that partially discrete models with clinically relevant subgroups embedded within a dimensional continuum is the most accurate characterization of some forms of psychopathology (Helzer et al., 2006, Krueger and Bezdjian, 2009). In particular, clinical phenomena such as substance use measured in non-clinical samples are characterized by ‘zero-inflation’, in which there are a large number of individuals with little-to-no symptoms (B. Muthén, 2006). Hybrid models, combining features of categorical and dimensional psychopathology, are a promising alternative approach to psychiatric research (Feczko et al., 2019, Krueger et al., 2018, Krueger and Bezdjian, 2009). Factor mixture modelling (FMM) is a type of latent variable analysis that combines the common factor modelling approach with latent class analysis (LCA) (Borsboom et al., 2016, Clark et al., 2013).

The approach can be used for identifying discrete, latent (i.e., not directly observed) classes or clinical subtypes embedded within multivariate dimensional data, including zero-inflation (Borsboom et al., 2016). Specification of a priori subtypes provides a natural demarcation in multivariate space that substantially reduce the dimensionality of the data and renders analysis of complex phenotypic data more tractable (Feczko et al., 2019). Hybrid models, such as FMM, may be important for identifying clinically meaningful subtypes with implications for informing earlier targeted interventions for those with ‘preaddiction’.

In terms of what this proposed continuum of substance use frequency reflects neurobiologically, there are several possibilities. Functional neuroimaging analysis of human decision-making has converged on a collection of cortical and subcortical brain regions involved in value-setting and intertemporal choice, (the preferential selection of rewards based on both magnitude and delay until obtainment) (Hamilton et al., 2015): the Valuation (VS), Executive Control (ECS) and Prospection (PS) Systems. The VS (ventromedial prefrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, amygdala and the posterior cingulate cortex) encodes the subjective values of various options during decision-making (Kable and Glimcher, 2007, Kable and Glimcher, 2010, Laurent et al., 2015, Peters and Buchel, 2010b), as well as generating goal-directed drug-seeking urges (Berridge, 2012, Leyton and Vezina, 2014, Steketee and Kalivas, 2011, Wolf, 2016). The ECS (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, lateral prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex) inhibits impulsive responses (van den Bos & McClure, 2013) via the incorporation of past outcomes and future goals (Kim et al., 2009, Sutton and Barto, 1998). The PS (dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, precuneus and medial temporal lobe) is activated during episodic memory recall or simulation of potential future scenarios (Schacter et al., 2007) involving drug use behaviour (Fang et al., 2021, Karch et al., 2015) and is thought to be associated with a preference for delayed rewards (Lempert et al., 2019). It is worth noting that regions of the VS (parts of the medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortices) and PS (middle temporal lobe, middle prefrontal cortex and precuneus) overlap with those recruited by the default mode network (Alshelh et al., 2018). However, the terms VS and PS are used for clarity as they better define the roles played by these regions with respect to value-based decision-making and delay discounting.

During dependency, addictive substances may enable the achievement of desired states (e.g., euphoria or pain relief). This is underpinned by the activation of, and interactions between, the VS, ECS and PS (Loganathan & Ho, 2021). The instrumental pursuit of addictive drugs can then lead to the development of choice impulsivity (Oberlin et al., 2021), the preferential selection of smaller, more immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards (Hamilton et al., 2015). Research indicates a significant relationship between delay discounting (a measure of choice impulsivity) and substance use behaviour in both adolescents and adults (Audrain-McGovern et al., 2009, Khurana et al., 2013, Khurana et al., 2017). A recent systematic review of task-based connectivity correlates with delay discounting behaviour (Owens et al., 2019) highlighted studies which showed positive correlation between functional connectivity and stronger delay discounting (i.e. greater impulsive choice) (Clewett et al., 2014, Contreras-Rodriguez et al., 2015) among cocaine and tobacco dependents. Particularly, connections within the fronto-parietal network (i.e., lateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices) were positively-correlated with steeper discounting among tobacco-smokers (Clewett et al., 2014). Stronger connections were observed between the caudate and anterior cingulate cortex in correlation with steeper discounting in cocaine dependents (Contreras-Rodriguez et al., 2015). While these studies must be praised for linking task-based delay discounting data with functional connectivity among substance dependents, an over-arching limitation remains the focus on a single substance rather than considering poly-substance use behaviour.

Turning the focus to resting-state fMRI studies, it has been observed that under normal circumstances, the interaction between VS and ECS is balanced, regulating impulsivity by incorporating phases of well-deliberated, disciplined thought and action (Dalley et al., 2011, Xie et al., 2014; T. Zhai et al., 2015). However, during dependency, VS activation appears to no longer be counterbalanced by the ECS, biasing decision-making towards the pursuit of drugs (Xie et al., 2014; T. Zhai et al., 2015). Interestingly, the PS has been implicated in reducing impulsive choice (Peters & Buchel, 2010a) by counteracting the growing predisposition towards more impulsive choices (VS) and concomitant reduced cognitive control (ECS) (Li et al., 2015, Liu et al., 2018, Verdejo-Garcia and Bechara, 2009, Xie et al., 2011; T.-Y. Zhai et al., 2014; T. Zhai et al., 2015). Participants living with cocaine dependence showed significant activation in the middle and superior frontal cortices, anterior cingulate, striatum and midbrain during a loss-chase task (Worhunsky et al., 2017), suggesting an integration of signals from regions of the VS, ECS and PS that contribute to choice impulsivity. These findings indicate that increased participation of valuation and decision-making regions may be required when losing gambles. Connectivity within the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala (both VS) during cocaine dependency suggests that increased value has been placed on this stimulant as a reward of choice (Contreras-Rodríguez et al., 2016). Additionally, connections between the nucleus accumbens (VS) and parts of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ECS) were weakened during dependency, reflecting reduced levels of behavioural control (Berlingeri et al., 2017, Motzkin et al., 2014). These individuals also expressed increased connectivity between regions of the anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (both ECS) with middle temporal and superior frontal gyri (both PS), indicating increased cognitive resources required to implement cognitive control (Camchong et al., 2011). Among alcohol abstainers, an inverse activation synchronicity between the nucleus accumbens (VS) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (ECS) suggests that increased resources are allocated to regulate behaviour away from alcohol consumption. Furthermore, reduced activation of the nucleus accumbens reflects restricted reward processing, possibly as a result of increased dorsolateral prefrontal cortex involvement (Camchong et al., 2013). Increased functional connectivity between the amygdala (VS) and both the frontal cortices (ECS and PS) as well as posterior parietal cortex (PPC) may trigger established drug-seeking behaviour and contribute to relapse (Kohno et al., 2017).

While decision-making remains functional during dependence, it is now heavily skewed towards drugs, despite higher costs. Dependence results in activation of the posterior cingulate, nucleus accumbens, medial temporal lobe, amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex, associated with willingness to pay more for drugs compared to non-drug items (Lawn et al., 2019). When challenged to pay more for increased drug doses, activations in the posterior cingulate cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex suggested that the subjective value, attentional orientation and intentionality of drugs may have taken precedence (Bedi et al., 2015; J. C. Gray et al., 2017; J. C. Gray & MacKillop, 2014). These results suggest changes in activation and functional connectivity between the VS, PS and ECS associated with substance dependency. What has not been established is whether quantitative changes in the activation and functional connectivity of these networks are observed prior to onset of dependence and index risk for developing SUDs. Additionally, there are pronounced neurotoxic effects of drugs of dependence which confounds neuroimaging studies of susceptibility biomarkers (Cunha-Oliveira et al., 2008, Gonçalves et al., 2014, Jacobus and Tapert, 2013, Squeglia et al., 2014). Thus, investigation of neural susceptibility biomarkers for SUDs in a normative population is essential to avoid the confounding effects of substance-induced neurotoxicity (Ersche et al., 2020, Ma et al., 2015).

Here, we propose investigation of neural susceptibility biomarkers for SUDs in a normative sample of young adults drawn from the Human Connectome Project (HCP). In order to deal with variations in substance use history and frequency while taking into account the mix-and-match tendency of users when consuming addictive substances (Scott et al., 2007), we propose a two-stage study. The first stage involves characterizing a dimensional phenotype of substance use data using FMM. In dimensional models of psychopathology, substance use represents a homogenous dimension combining use across alcohol, marijuana, and other drug classes (Krueger and South, 2009, Patrick et al., 2013). Thus, we expected a unidimensional substance use continuum capturing covariance in the frequency of use of all substance classes, including alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, stimulants, sedatives, and opiates. We expected a 2-class model to best capture the data, consisting of a zero-inflated class in which participants uniformly endorsed low frequency of use across all substance classes and a class in which there was a continuous dimension of substance use frequency (B. Muthén, 2006). This continuum will then form the foundation for the second stage of the study: identifying brain regions and functional connections of the VS, ECS and PS that are positively-correlated with the substance use continuum among HCP participants using the Network-based Statistic (NBS) (Zalesky et al., 2010). The NBS is a robust network neuroscience tool for mapping connections between brain regions to produce subnetworks of brain regions correlated with the contrast of interest (such as cognitive task scores or substance use behaviour). One of the biggest advantages of the NBS is its ability to correct for family-wise errors at a network level, resulting in subnetworks that have a lower probability of being classified as false discoveries (Zalesky et al., 2010, Zalesky et al., 2012). Furthermore, given that sensation-seeking is a major dimension of both impulsivity construct (Norbury & Husain, 2015) and substance use (Owens et al., 2019) we further hypothesize that while a positive correlation exists between the continuum (i.e., more frequent drug use) and network functional connectivity, a negative correlation with delay discounting scores (i.e., greater impulsivity) may be observed. Taken another way, connectivity within and between regions of the VS, ECS and PS are hypothesized to be stronger as substance use and impulsive behaviour increases.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants and measures

A total of 1200 participants (652 females), aged between 22 and 37 years old, were sampled from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) Young Adult dataset. Recruitment and inclusion/exclusion criteria are described elsewhere (Van Essen et al., 2013). Briefly, the HCP consortium defined ‘healthy’ in broad terms so as to generate a pool of subjects representative of the population at large, capturing a wide range of variability in healthy individuals with respect to behavioural, ethnic, and socioeconomic diversity. They excluded individuals having severe neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., autism), documented neuropsychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia or depression) or neurologic disorders (e.g., Parkinson's disease). They also excluded individuals with illnesses such as diabetes or high blood pressure, as these might negatively impact neuroimaging data quality. Additionally, they included individuals who are smokers, and/or have a history of heavy drinking or recreational drug use without having experienced severe symptoms to facilitate connectivity studies on psychiatric patients who have subclinical substance use behaviours (Van Essen et al., 2013).

For this study, 6 subjects were excluded from the original 1206 participants due to having incomplete records of substance use behaviour, as well as other cognitive assessments. Variables of interest were SSAGA (Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism) Alcohol DSM4 Abuse Diagnosis (i.e., Does the participant meet the DSM4 criteria for Alcohol Abuse sometime over his/her lifetime?), SSAGA FTND Score (Fagerstrom FTND test for nicotine dependence), SSAGA Times Used Cocaine, SSAGA Times Used Hallucinogens, SSAGA Times Used Opiates, SSAGA Times Used Sedatives, SSAGA Times Used Stimulants, times used marijuana, as well as age and sex. These SSAGA Assessments were performed before any brain scans were collected, as part of an early screening process to ensure all prospective participants met HCP inclusion criterion (Van Essen et al., 2012, Van Essen et al., 2013). All variables were categorical. Missing SSAGA FTND data were imputed using the k-nearest neighbour method (Malarvizhi & Thanamani, 2012). The authors applied for access to participants’ drug use records and parental use history via the University of Melbourne’s Research Innovation and Commercialization department. This request was granted by the Human Connectome Project Consortium.

2.2. Factor mixture modelling

Substance use data were available for 1,200 participants, whereas only 1,008 participants had neuroimaging data. We chose to conduct the factor mixture modelling (FMM) on the total sample (N = 1,200), because statistical techniques that test for latent classes embedded in multivariate data are sensitive to sample size, such that larger samples enable more complex models to be tested (Nylund-Gibson & Choi, 2018). Additionally, analyses of multivariate data should use all available data to avoid converging on biased estimates (Enders, 2010). For these reasons, we chose to analyse all available data from the HCP.

FMM is a combination of latent class analysis and factor analysis. In latent class analysis (LCA), one or more (k) unobserved classes (C) explains the observed pattern of responses on a set of observed variables (e.g., item responses on a questionnaire). Class assignment (i.e., class probabilities) of each participant is determined as posterior probabilities based on the observed response pattern (see Clark et al., 2013 for details). The observed variables are assumed to be conditionally independent of each other after the response pattern is explained by the latent class variable (see Fig. 1a). Each participant is allowed fractional class membership and may have non-zero probabilities of being in multiple classes. However, participants are assigned to a specific class based on the highest posterior probability. A summary measure of classification accuracy of participants based on the posterior probabilities of class membership within an LCA and FMM is provided by the entropy (E), which ranges between 0.00 and 1.00, with higher entropy indicating better classification accuracy (Clark & Muthén, 2009). When entropy is high (e.g., ≥0.80) class membership can be used as a discrete categorical variable for subsequent analyses to compare results between classes (Clark & Muthén, 2009). Classes must be compared using alternative analytic approaches that take into account the probabilistic nature of class membership when entropy is low (Nylund-Gibson et al., 2019). However, the limitation of LCA is that classes are assumed to be homogenous, such that participants within the same class are assumed to have the same scores.

Fig. 1.

Fig 1

Model diagrams of a) latent class analysis; b) factor analysis; and c) factor mixture modelling. Adapted from Clark et al. (Clark et al., 2013). Note. Boxes represent observed categorical variables. u1 - ur = item responses on a questionnaire (e.g., SSAGA). Circles represent unobserved (i.e., latent variables). C = latent unordered class variable with k discrete classes. f = continuous factor / latent variable. Straight single-headed arrows indicate causal paths. Dashed straight lines indicate conditional independence of item responses after being explained by the latent variables C and/or f. Small diagonal arrow pointing to the factor in 1c is the factor variance.

In factor analysis, the pattern of observed responses is explained by a continuous latent variable called a factor (f) (see Fig. 1b). Observed responses are assumed to be conditionally independent once their covariance is explained by the common factor (see Clark et al., 2013 for details). For individual participants, each observed variable is decomposed into a combination of elements, including an intercept, a factor loading determining the influence of a factor on the measured variable, and the unique variance/error of the measured variable that is not explained by the factor loading (Bollen & Noble, 2011). The factor loadings capture the shared variance across the items explained by the factor. Participants are not assumed to be comprised of two or more subpopulations, but rather differences in pattern responses are determined by differences on the underlying factor. Thus, participants can be rank-ordered along a continuous dimension of the factor, which can be expressed through the derivation of observed variables called factor score estimates (Grice, 2001). Factor score estimates are an approximation of the sample distribution of the factor and express where each individual is located on the factor relative to the rest of the sample (Clark et al., 2013). However, a limitation of factor analysis is that participants in a sample are assumed to be from the same subpopulation with no qualitative or quantitative differences in the structure of the factor on which they are rank ordered.

FMM is a hybrid approach that combines the features of LCA and factor analysis (see Fig. 1c). The pattern of observed variables (i.e., item responses) is determined both by one or more latent classes and one or more factors (as indicated by the solid single-headed straight arrows) (see Clark et al., 2013 for details). Moreover, the factor means and variances (as indicated by the solid straight-headed arrow pointing from C to f) and factor loadings (as indicated by the dashed single-headed arrows) are allowed to vary as a function of class membership, which adds a great deal of flexibility to the model. FMM allows for the characterization of heterogeneity with latent classes by modeling the continuous factor and allowing the derivation of factor score estimates to quantify individual differences in scores within each class.

There are four variations of the FMM, ordered from the most restrictive to the least restrictive (i.e., the most to the least model parameters fixed to equality across classes) (see Fig. 2a – 2d). In the most restrictive model, the FMM-1, factor variances, observed variable thresholds (i.e., for categorical variables) or intercepts (i.e., for continuous variables), as well as factor loadings are fixed to equality across classes. This model suggests only differences in factor means across classes as would be expected for a non-normally distributed latent variable. For the FMM-2, factor variances are free to vary across classes, such that the factors are measured equivalently along the same continuum (i.e., factor means can be compared), but have different distributions. For the FMM-3, observed variable thresholds or intercepts vary across classes, suggesting differences in observed variables independent of differences in the factor (e.g., systematic response biases on questionnaire items within classes/groups), such that factor means can no longer be meaningfully compared across classes. Finally, for FMM-4, factor variances, thresholds/intercepts and factor loadings all vary across classes, such that the factors are no longer measured equivalently and do not have the same substantive interpretation across classes (e.g., items in a questionnaire are differentially related to the factor) (see Clark et al., 2013 for details).

Fig. 2.

Fig 2

Model diagrams of different types of factor mixture models: a) FMM-1 – class membership determines differences in factor means (ak) only; b) FMM-2 – class membership determines differences in factor means and factor variances (as indicated by small single-headed diagonal arrow pointing to f; and c) FMM-3 – class membership determines observed variable thresholds/intercepts and the factor variance–covariance matrix is also free to vary across classes); FMM-4 – class membership determines factor loadings, observed variable thresholds/intercepts and the factor variance–covariance matrix is also free to vary across classes) Adapted from Clark et al. (Clark et al., 2013). Note. Boxes represent observed categorical variables. u1 - ur = item responses on a questionnaire (e.g., SSAGA). Circles represent unobserved (i.e., latent variables). C = latent unordered class variable with k discrete classes. f = continuous factor / latent variable. Straight single-headed arrows indicate causal paths. Dashed straight lines indicate conditional independence of item responses after being explained by the latent variables C and/or f.

We conducted the analyses following the procedure outline by Clark et al. (Clark et al., 2013) and using the Mplus program version 8.3 (L. K. Muthén, 2017). First, we fit factor analysis and latent class models to the SSAGA data for later comparison and to determine the upper bound for the number of factors and classes for the factor mixture models (Clark et al., 2013). For factor analysis, we used exploratory structural equation modelling (ESEM) and the weighted least square mean- and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimator, which is the preferred estimator with ordered categorical (i.e., ordinal) data (Byrne et al., 2012; B. Muthén et al., 1997; L. Muthén & Muthén, 2017). ESEM is a hybrid of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, which takes an exploratory approach to modelling whilst also enabling model-data consistency to be evaluated with fit statistics (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2009, Marsh et al., 2014). We used a competing models strategy to determine whether the most parsimonious unidimensional model provided a superior fit compared to models with two or more factors (Hair et al., 2014a, Jöreskog, 1993). As it was possible that local dependencies between subsets of items could cause poor fit for a unifactorial model, we considered freely estimating correlated residuals (θδ) where consistent with theory and if significant after correction for Type I error using the Benjamini-Hochberg procedure (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995, Silvia and MacCallum, 1988). To evaluate model-data consistency, we used a combination of absolute and approximate global fit statistics, as well as indices of local fit. We referred to the chi square (χ2) test statistic first, where p >.05 suggests the exact fit hypothesis for model-data consistency cannot be rejected (Hayduk et al., 2007, Marsh et al., 2004). We also report three approximate fit indices, the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Higher values for the CFI, and lower values of the RMSEA and associated 90% confidence interval (90 %CI), and SRMR are indicative of better fitting models. (Barrett, 2007, Byrne, 2013, Hair et al., 2014, Hayduk et al., 2007). To evaluate local fit, we used the matrix of correlation residuals (ε), which reveal discrepancies in the model estimated and observed bivariate correlations; where a pattern of ε greater than 0.10 indicates potential sources of poor local fit (Kline, 2015).

For latent class analysis (LCA), we used the maximum likelihood estimator with a chi square statistic and standard errors robust to non-normality (MLR) to handle the ordinal data. To determine the optimal number of classes in LCA, we followed the procedure outlined by Nylund et al. and Asparouhov and Muthen (Asparouhov and Muthén, 2009, Nylund et al., 2007). We generated 1–5-class models examining the inflection points for the trend in the log likelihood and Bayesian information criterion (BIC) values to identify a smaller range of plausible models (Nylund et al., 2007). From this smaller range of candidate models, the best log likelihood values were obtained for each number of classes tested using an initial number of random starting value perturbations and final stage optimizations (160, 32). The model was then rerun with double the number of random starting value perturbations and final stage optimizations (320, 64) to ensure that the analyses did not converge on local maxima in estimating the best log likelihood value (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2012; L. Muthén & Muthén, 2017).

Once the best log likelihood was replicated, each model was rerun to obtain the Lo–Mendell–Rubin (LMR) adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test (LRT) and Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT) by using the seed that resulted in the best log likelihood value specified as the starting value instead of random starts. Class enumeration was evaluated using a combination of fit statistics, including the entropy (E), BIC (Hair et al., 2014, Schwarz, 1978), the LMR adjusted LRT, and BLRT (Lo et al., 2001). Entropy is ranked from 0.00 to 1.00, with higher values indicating better class separation (Clark & Muthén, 2009). Lower BIC values indicate a better-fitting and more parsimonious model (Clark & Muthén, 2009). A non-significant p value for the LMR adjusted LRT and BLRT indicates that the k – 1 class model provides a better fit to the data than the k model or any subsequent k + 1 models (Nylund et al., 2007). The combination of these statistics has been determined to provide a relatively sensitive measure of the true number of classes (Nylund et al., 2007). Comparative model performance was also evaluated using the Bayesian conditional posterior probability, which quantifies the relative probability (p =.00–1.0) of model i compared to k models by dividing the exponentiated -12BIC for model i by the sum of the exponentiated -12BIC for k models:P⁢rB⁢I⁢C⁢(𝐻𝑖⁡|𝐷)=e⁢x⁢p⁡[−12⁢B⁢I⁢C⁢(𝐻𝑖)]∑𝑘−1𝑗=0e⁢x⁢p⁡[−12⁢B⁢I⁢C⁢(𝐻𝑗)](Wagenmakers, 2007).

After determining the optimal number of classes, we then proceeded to test factor mixture models (FMM), using the MLR estimator to handle the ordinal data (L. Muthén & Muthén, 2017). We began with one-factor one-class and one-factor two-class models (Clark et al., 2013) and the most restrictive and parsimonious factor mixture model (i.e., FMM-1, different latent means only) before progressively relaxing equality constraints on the factor variance–covariance matrix (i.e., FMM-2); the item thresholds (i.e., FMM-3), and the factor loadings (i.e., FMM-4) to determine the best fitting model as indicated by the log likelihoods, entropy, BIC, and P⁢rB⁢I⁢C⁢(𝐻𝑖⁡|𝐷)(Clark et al., 2013).We then systematically increased the number of specified classes for the one-factor model until reaching the k number of classes for the besting fitting LCA model. Finally, we compared the best-fitting factor mixture model to the best factor model and best latent class model using the BIC to determine the optimal representation of the data (Clark et al., 2013).

2.3. Delay discounting

In the Human Connectome Project (HCP), delay discounting was calculated for each individual using an extra-scanner Area Under Curve (AUC) approach, a model-free method that describes the delay discounting tendency of an individual (Green and Myerson, 2004, Myerson et al., 2001). Delays to reward-obtainment are fixed, but reward amounts are adjusted on a trial-by-trial basis based on a participant’s previous choice until an indifference point is reached. This represents the delay margin when the participant is more likely to choose a smaller but more immediate reward over a larger but delayed one, the theoretical indifference between the delayed reward and the estimated present subjective value of said reward to produce a discount curve using methods such as the AUC (Borges et al., 2016, Hamilton et al., 2015). The AUC for each participant is the total area of all trapezoids in his/her discounting curve (Frost & McNaughton, 2017). For more information, please see the Supplementary Methods.

2.4. Correlation between value-based decision-making network functional connectivity and substance use

Minimally preprocessed functional MRI data from the Human Connectome Project (HCP, Smith et al., 2013) was sourced for healthy adults of both genders (age range = 22–30). Only subjects with all four repeated resting-state fMRI sessions (first and second scan sessions with left–right and right-left phase encoding directions), who also possessed complete delay discounting, cognition, socioeconomic scores (i.e., education, income, and employment status) and mental health (i.e., depression, anxiety and somatic) measures, were included (n = 650). For a full account of the HCP neuroimaging acquisition and pre-preprocessing parameters, please see the Supplementary Methods.

The VS, ECS and PS brain masks were delineated using binary masks that combined regions of interest (ROIs) from both the Desikan-Killiany (Desikan et al., 2006) and Destrieux (Destrieux et al., 2010) parcellations (Table S1 in the Supplementary Methods and Results). All anatomical labels were extracted and merged using the FMRIB Software Library (Smith et al., 2004, https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/). Resting-state functional connectivity scans in the HCP are divided into 4 subsets of scans, REST1 left–right (L-R), REST 1 R-L (REST1 R-L), REST2 L-R, REST2 R-L. For each subset, the resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rsfMRI) signal was averaged over all voxels comprising each ROI (node). The Pearson correlation coefficients between the regionally averaged signals for all nodes were then computed for each subset of scans per participant (i.e., each participants’ REST1 L-R scans forming one subset of n-by-n matrices, each participants’ REST1 R-L scans forming one subset of n-by-n matrices, and so on). To identify functional circuits within the VS, ECS and PS associated with Substance Use factor score estimates using the Network-Based Statistic (Zalesky et al., 2010), design matrices comprised of age (in years), sex, framewise displacement, transformed rates of discounting, cognitive scores, socioeconomic measures (i.e. employment, education and income), DSM-IV diagnosis of mental health conditions (i.e., depression, anxiety and somatic symptom disorder) and Substance Use factor score estimates as well as their standard errors, were correlated with the connectivity matrices of the VS, ECS and PS. Standard errors of the factor score estimates were included because measurement precision of the substance use factor was not uniform across the latent trait continuum due to the unipolar nature of the substance use construct (i.e., SSAGA items provide measurement of the presence of substance use problems, but there are no items that provide measurement of the low end of the continuum, see Figure S1). The measurement error is proportional to the distributional properties of the signal (i.e., a ‘multiplicative error-in-variable model’). However, this relationship is not monotonic. Thus, including the standard errors adjusts for this non-uniform measurement precision across the latent trait continuum. A composite subnetwork featuring edges that were common across all four subsets brain scans was compiled and visualized using BrainNet Viewer. This approach may help avoid issues of variability between visits (i.e. between REST1 and REST2) and hemispheric lateralization (Cao et al., 2021, Korponay and Koenigs, 2021, Ocklenburg and Mundorf, 2022). A family-wise error (FWE)-corrected p-value was calculated to identify the largest interconnected cluster of brain regions (5000 permutations) at a threshold of t = 2.5, p <.01. This threshold was selected from a range of possible values (1.0 to 5.0), the suitability of each was tested iteratively in increments of 0.5. Anything higher (i.e., greater than 3.0) resulted in nearly all edges no longer having a measure of association higher than the pre-set threshold. Put another way, thresholds above 2.5 would remove nearly all connections, leaving a subnetwork so sparse as to have almost no connections between brain regions. Pairwise connections were then visualized with BrainNet Viewer (Xia et al., 2013).

3. Results

3.1. Modelling of substance use behaviour

We found that a one-factor model, with two freely estimate error covariances (marijuana use with tobacco use θδ = 0.306, SE = 0.050 [95 %CI = 0.209, 0.403], p <.001 & hallucinogens θδ = 0.489, SE = 0.065 [95 %CI = 0.361, 0.617], p <.001) provided the best and most parsimonious representation of the latent structure of the data using ESEM (χ2 (18) = 26.069, p =.098, RMSEA = 0.019 [90 %CI = 0.000, 0.035], CFI = 0.998, SRMR = 0.025; see Fig. 3). Frequency of use for all substance classes loaded onto the common ‘Substance Use’ factor at p <.001 and there was only one correlation residual > 0.10 suggesting that the association been alcohol and tobacco had been slightly underestimated (see Table 1). In contrast, the competing two, three-, or four-factor models were all misspecified resulting in error warnings and indicating that they did not capture the data well. The results of the LCA suggested that a three-class model provided the best fit compared to one-, two-, or four-class models (see Table 2) whereas a five-class model was misspecified. The results of the LCA provide an upper bound for the number of classes that can be expected to fit the data for the FMM. This is because the LCA does not take account of the dimensional structure of the data and thus overestimates the number of classes (i.e., it takes more classes to fit the data than are needed because the factor structure of the variables is ignored). Thus, the three-class model represented the upper bound of the number of classes for the FMM.

Fig. 3.

Fig 3

One-factor model of substance use in the Human Connectome Project participants. χ2(18) = 26.069, p =.098, RMSEA = 0.019 [90 %CI = 0.000, 0.035], CFI = 0.998, SRMR = 0.025. All factor loadings, as well as the two residual correlations, were statistically significant p <.001 N = 1,200.

Table 1. Correlation Residuals for the Two-Factor Model of Self-Reported Compulsivity as Modeled with the WLSMV Estimator.

Table 1

Note. Bold typeface denotes correlation residuals (ε) ≥ 0.100.

Table 2. Results of Exploratory Latent Class Analysis of Substance Use in Human Connectome Participants.

Table 2

Note. ASRS-5 = The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Screening Scale for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders – Fifth Edition (DSM-5). LL = log likelihood; LR = likelihood ratio. LR Δ2df = degrees of freedom for the likelihood ratio chi-square test. LR Δ2 = Likelihood ratio chi-square test of the difference between the observed versus expected frequency tables for the categorical latent class indicators. LR Δ2p = probability value for the likelihood ratio chi-square test. LMR = Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test when comparing the k to k – 1 class model; LMR p = probability value for the Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test. Δq = difference in the number of parameters between comparison models. 2*ΔLL = Two times the log likelihood difference between k and k – 1 models for the bootstrapped likelihood ratio test. BLRT p = probability value for the bootstrapped likelihood ratio test. BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; Pr (Hi | D) = Bayesian conditional posterior probability of k model compared to all other estimated models. N = 1,200.

Five class model failed to converge on trustworthy estimates.

Bold typeface denotes preferred model based on converging evidence across fit statistics.

1Best loglikelihood values initially obtained using 160 and 32, then replicated using 320 and 64, random starting value perturbations and final stage optimizations.

28 and 4 starting values and final stage optimizations for the k-1 model and 320 and 64 starting values and final stage optimizations for the k model.

We then estimated FMMs (FMM-1 to FMM-4) with the unidimensional Substance Use factor and one- to three-class models (see Table 1). We also estimated these models specifying a zero-inflated class with factor loadings and factor variances fixed at zero, and factor means freely estimated and the starting values of the item thresholds set to low probability of endorsement. Finally, we estimated two- and three-class non-parametric FMMs with factor variances fixed at zero. These models are indicated when the distributions of the factor are non-normal, such as the zero-inflated distribution of clinical variables in non-clinical populations, including substance use in the current sample. Most of these models failed to converge on trustworthy estimates, indicating misspecified (i.e., ill-fitting) models. A two-class model with class varying factor variances and thresholds (FMM-3) provided a reasonable fit to the data, although class separation was relatively poor (LL = -4,328.817, E = 0.707, BIC = 10168.916). However, the one-class one-factor model provided a superior fit to the data (LL = 4,901.545, BIC = 10086.602) as revealed by the Bayes factor, which provided very strong evidence in favour of this model compared to the two-class FMM-3 (BF01 = 7.486140810132e+17).

The latent variable distribution plot is provided in Fig 4 and indicates some zero-inflation in the distribution. We generated factor score estimates using the regression method (Grice, 2001, Muthén and Muthén, n.d..) for each participant based on the one-class, one-factor solution for subsequent analysis with the neuroimaging data. The information function for the Substance Use latent variable is shown in Figure S2. Information (I) can be converted into a standard metric of reliability (rxx) using the formula [𝑟𝑥𝑥=1−(1/𝐼)]and is plotted in standardized units along the latent trait continuum (i.e., M = 0, SD = 1) (Toland, 2014). Not surprisingly, measurement precision was highest above the mean where it was approximately rxx = 0.6, peaking at + 2SD (rxx = 0.93), before dropping below rxx = 0.6 again at ∼+3.5SD. This was due to the unipolar nature of the construct ‘Substance Use’ (i.e., there is no item content to measure the adaptive end of the continuum (Lucke, 2015) beyond low substance use).

Fig. 4.

Fig 4

Latent variance distribution plot for the ‘Substance Use’ factor. N = 1,200. The × axis is defined in standardized units, with a mean of zero and standard deviation of one. The metric of the y axis is the number of participants in the sample with that value of the latent variable.

We also regressed the Substance Use factor onto sex, age, maternal and paternal substance use history to determine if these demographic variables explained variance in the substance use behaviour of participants (see Figure S2). We found that males (γ = -0.249, SE = 0.033, p <.001) and older participants (γ = 0.065, SE = 0.033, p =.051) tended to have higher substance use, as did those participants whose mother (γ = 0.097, SE = 0.032, p =.002) and father (γ = 0.103, SE = 0.031, p =.001) had a substance use history. However, the effect sizes were very small (R2 ≤ 0.011), except for sex (R2 = 0.062). The brant Wald test for proportional odds was only significant for sex and marijuana use (χ2(4) = 15.173, p =.004), indicating that the pattern of endorsement of frequency of use was different between males and females.

Note. Model figure is displayed using symbols from the McArdle-McDonald reticular action model (RAM) (McArdle, 1980). Observed (also measured or manifest) variables are represented as rectangles. The factor (latent variable or construct) is represented as a large ellipse. The double-headed, curved arrow pointing to the factor is the latent variable variance. Straight, single-headed arrows from the large ellipse to observed variables reflect factor loadings. Curved, double-headed arrows between rectangles are error covariances/residual correlations.

3.2. Network-based Statistic modelling

Significant subnetworks were observed when functional connectivity of regions recruited by the VS, ECS and PS as well as the insula, caudate, putamen and intraparietal sulcus were correlated with the design matrix. All subnetworks were negatively correlated with delay discounting, cognitive scores, employment, education and income status but positively correlated with DSM-IV diagnoses of depression, anxiety and somatic problems, as well as substance use factor score estimates and their standard errors. Given that this NBS analyses was performed independently on each subset of brain scans (i.e. REST1 L-R, REST1 R-L and so on), four separate significant subnetworks were obtained. A composite list was then compiled consisting entirely of connections found across all four subsets. This composite list of connections is represented graphically in Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Fig 5

Composite connections found in each of the four significant (p < 0.05) subnetworks identified by the NBS from subsets of HCP resting state brain scans (i.e., REST 1 L-R, REST1 R-L, REST2 L-R, REST2 R-L). All subnetworks were negatively correlated with delay discounting, cognitive scores, employment, education, and income status but positively correlated with DSM-IV diagnoses of depression, anxiety, and somatic problems as well as substance use factor score estimates and their standard errors. (A) Functional connections within the ECS are positively correlated with Substance Use factor score estimates; (B) VS functional connections with regions of the ECS and PS were positively correlated with Substance Use factor score estimates; (C) ECS functional connections with regions of the PS were positively correlated with Substance Use factor score estimates.

Connections involving regions of the VS were left medial OFC to the left superior parietal and left anterior middle cingulate as well as the right ventral posterior cingulate to the right anterior middle cingulate, right posterior middle cingulate and bilateral insula. Connections involving regions of the ECS were the bilateral superior parietal cortex to the right posterior middle cingulate, the bilateral rostral middle cingulate to the right posterior middle cingulate, the right posterior middle cingulate to the bilateral precuneus, left rostral middle frontal to right precuneus, left superior parietal and right posterior middle cingulate to the bilateral superior frontal cortex, left superior parietal, right posterior middle cingulate, bilateral rostral middle frontal and bilateral caudal middle frontal to the right insula and right rostral middle frontal to left posterior middle cingulate. Connections involving regions of the PS were the bilateral precuneus to the left superior frontal cortex and right insula, as well as the left superior frontal cortex to the right insula.

4. Discussion

The aim of our study was to identify neuroimaging susceptibility biomarkers of SUDs in young adults from the Human Connectome Project (HCP). Rather than being taxonomic in structure, the continuity hypothesis suggests that there is a continuum of substance use extending from normal to problematic use in the population (Borsboom et al., 2016). This continuity has previously been demonstrated for alcohol use (Krueger et al., 2004). However, it also possible that latent classes are embedded within the continuum of substance use problems suggesting that hybrid models that combine dimensional and categorical measurement may better fit the data (B. Muthén, 2006). Here we used factor mixture modelling, a hybrid modelling approach that combines factor analysis and latent class analysis, which is well-suited to characterizing the latent structure of psychiatric phenomena, such as substance use behaviour (Clark et al., 2013, Miettunen et al., 2016). We hypothesized that a single continuum of substance use frequency and severity with a distinct zero-inflated latent class would best characterize the substance use behaviour in our sample of young adults from the HCP.

Our hypothesis of a single substance use continuum that combined frequency of use across all substance classes was supported. Each of the substances loaded moderately (alcohol and tobacco) to strongly (marijuana, hallucinogens, sedatives, opiates, stimulants, cocaine) on a unidimensional factor, suggesting that a single continuum adequately captures and describes covariation of substance use across all classes within the sample. In contrast, hybrid models with two or more latent classes, including a zero-inflated class characterizing a proportion of individuals with low frequency of use across all substance classes, failed to provide a good fit to the data. Thus, despite evidence of some zero-inflation in the continuum of substance use behaviour, a hybrid model with a distinct class of individuals with low scores did not provide a good fit to the data as compared to a model with a single continuous dimension. These findings indicated that young adults from the HCP could be rank-ordered along a single continuum of substance use frequency and severity. Furthermore, findings indicated that factor score estimates generated from this continuum could be analyzed with functional connectivity data to examine brain-behaviour associations and identify the functional neural substrates of substance use behaviour. Such dimensional analyses are better situated to detect meaningful brain-behaviour associations compared to categorical distinctions and speak to the utility of a dimensional enhancement approach to researching SUDs (Cuthbert, 2014).

4.1. Functional connectivity within and between the VS, ECS, and PS is associated with substance use behaviour

We found that connections between the VS, ECS and PS were positively correlated with substance use factor score estimates, as well as incidence of depression, anxiety, and somatic problems, but was negatively correlated with delay discounting (i.e., choice impulsivity). These findings agree with our previously stated hypotheses and can be interpreted in the context of reward-based valuation and decision making (i.e., sensation seeking), which represents a predisposing factor for SUDs (Verdejo-Garcia & Albein-Urios, 2021). Sensation-seeking utilizes a goal-directed approach system (J. A. Gray, 1990) geared towards satisfying the need for rewarding experiences (Zuckerman, 1994). This behaviour is a part of the impulsivity construct (Miranda-Olivos et al., 2022) and may motivate substance use (including poly-substance use) (Chakroun et al., 2004, Woicik et al., 2009). Regions of the ECS (and PS) could provide goal-directed value signals to the medial OFC (VS), aiding decision-making in pursuit of addictive substances. Even among non-dependent drug users, functional activation (Filbey & Dunlop, 2014) and connectivity have been reported previously (Ersche et al., 2020). Both studies highlight the impact of regions such as the precuneus, superior frontal cortex (both PS), posterior parietal and lateral prefrontal cortex (ECS) in preventing a non-dependent drug user from becoming dependent. Connections between the VS and PS may contribute to the development of interoceptive thoughts revolving around the pleasurable sensations experienced during drug use, but the numerous connections with ECS regions could help to restrict the impact of these thoughts and the potential transition to dependence.

It has been theorized that a delicate balance may exist between valuation- and control-based regions of the brain that regulate impulsive behaviours, and that disruption of this balance may underpin the development of addictive behaviours (Xie et al., 2014; T. Zhai et al., 2015). More recent studies indicate that regions of the ECS (the lateral prefrontal cortex in the context of the current study) and PS (the superior frontal cortex, once again in the context of this study) are implicated in top-down inhibitory control (Ersche et al., 2020). Here, we observed a positive correlation between substance use factor score estimates and connections between regions of the VS, ECS and PS among poly-drug users. We thus infer that although these participants may favour mixing-and-matching various substances to heighten their consumptive experiences (connections between the VS and ECS, PS), they may not have developed full-on dependency owing to the protective effects of connections within and between the ECS (particularly the lateral prefrontal cortex) and PS (particularly the superior frontal cortex). The potential role of both the lateral prefrontal and superior frontal cortices in potentially staving off dependency may be further emphasised via the concomitant negative correlation between participants’ delay discounting scores and network connectivity. The more impulsive one becomes, the stronger the connections within and between the ECS and PS, a possible countermeasure to preserve the balance between the value of poly-drug use and top-down cognitive control.

The lateral prefrontal, posterior cingulate and posterior parietal cortices emerged as hubs within the ECS (Fig. 5). The lateral prefrontal cortex orients attention and subserves approach behaviour towards rewards, disregarding the consequences of risky behaviour during sensation-seeking (Cservenka et al., 2013, Davidson et al., 2004). Left lateral prefrontal cortex activation is associated with both impulsivity and sensation-seeking (Chase et al., 2017) by encoding stimulus-outcome associations (Boorman et al., 2016), preparatory attention (van Schouwenburg et al., 2010, Wallis et al., 2015, Woolgar et al., 2015), optimistic bias (Garrett et al., 2014), and free-choice (Cho et al., 2016). The posterior parietal cortex is associated with decision-making and is a reliable predictor of risk-taking (Gilaie-Dotan et al., 2014) and risk preference (Teti Mayer et al., 2021), while the posterior cingulate cortex is thought to encode the neuroeconomic value of the subject (in this case, the drug of choice) (Brosch et al., 2013). Connections from the posterior parietal cortex extend to the medial orbitofrontal cortex and superior frontal cortex, while the right posterior middle cingulate is connected to the superior frontal cortex and precuneus (Fig. 5B). These findings suggest that impulsive sensation-seekers willingly engage in goal-directed poly-drug use (risky behaviour), with the optimistic view that they will once again experience the same pleasurable sensations as before by consuming their preferred substance(s).

Interestingly, connections were observed between the insula and regions of the VS, ECS and PS. The insula is believed to be involved in attention orientation towards rewards (Anderson et al., 2016, Farrant and Uddin, 2015), being sensitive towards cues signalling preferred rewards (Goudriaan et al., 2010, Kober et al., 2016, Limbrick-Oldfield et al., 2013). Increased functional connectivity was observed between the insula and the ventral cingulate cortex (VS), as well as regions of the ECS (i.e., bilateral rostral and caudal middle frontal cortex, bilateral posterior middle cingulate cortex and left superior parietal cortex), as well as the PS (i.e., bilateral precuneus and left superior frontal cortex). These results suggest that drug users may have a fixation towards thoughts of drugs as the reward of choice (VS - posterior cingulate cortex), fuelled in part by memories of previous use and those outcomes (PS). This increased connectivity was negatively correlated with delay discounting scores (i.e., steeper discounting or a preference for risker choices), particularly reflected by connections between the insula and the prefrontal cortex. These results further suggests the influence of reward-related attentional bias on value-based decision-making, possibly making it more challenging to accept delayed rewards in the face of faster returns (Clewett et al., 2014). Nevertheless, connections involving ECS regions may balance poly-substance pursuit (and the perceived benefits of its consumption), thereby stalling or even preventing dependence.

Our findings can be interpreted from two perspectives. First, the relationship between delay discounting and functional connectivity among substance users. As mentioned earlier, Owens et al. (2019) performed a systematic review of the literature surrounding functional and structural connectivity among substance users and its relationship with delay discounting. Our results concur with some of the studies highlighted by Owens et al. For example, Camchong et al. (2011) reported increased connectivity between the anterior cingulate cortex and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex among cocaine dependents. Clewett et al. (2014) also reported similar findings among smokers, involving the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices correlated (Clewett et al., 2014). Contreras-Rodriguez et al. (2015) observed increased connectivity between the ventral striatum (site of the nucleus accumbens) and the anterior cingulate cortex. All three studies showed positive correlation with delay discounting, in contrast to our findings. While regions like the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices appear to act as hubs in our study, we utilized data for use patterns across eight different substances. As observed by Morris et al (2022) the presence of multiple substances, combined with increased use severity can cause changes in connectivity correlates. It may also reflect the increased value attributed to taking combinations of substances as a means of achieving pleasure or pain relief. Individuals may crave for their drug(s) of choice, and may be willing to accept smaller doses if it can be procured more quickly (Loganathan & Ho, 2021). They may also view mixing-and-matching addictive substances as a quick and easy way to reach their desired state, instead of utilizing non-substance related means.

The second perspective revolves around utilization of our findings. Research indicates that while most studies focus on chronic, severe substance abuse (i.e. addiction), a majority of the population experiences mild-to-moderate symptoms of substance use disorder and account for more substance-use harms compared to those with severe SUDs (Asken et al., 2007, McLellan et al., 2022). Recently, McLellan et al (2022) shone the spotlight on ‘preaddiction’, a term describing the state of mild-to-moderate substance use disorders that could pre-date addiction. They also highlight a lack of objective assessments to detect individuals in a preaddiction state (McLellan et al., 2022). We would like to propose the continuum reported here, along with the functional hubs and edges correlated with both factor score estimates of substance use behaviour and delay discounting scores, as a possible neurobiological assessment to be used with individuals in the preaddiction state. We further propose that our models be used in tandem with existing diagnostic criteria as contained within the DSM-5 (Asken et al., 2007) when assessing patients with substance use disorder.

The results of our study are tempered by methodological limitations. First, it is possible for mixture and hybrid modelling to yield solutions that are idiosyncratic to specific samples (Borsboom et al., 2016). Thus, it will be important to replicate the findings of a continuous, unidimensional spectrum of substance use frequency and severity in other samples and determine if the same neurobiological substrates are uncovered. Unfortunately, we did not have an independent sample with which to replicate our results. The one-factor, one-class solution we converged on may reflect the non-clinical characteristics of the sample, in which only a small subset of participants reported use of illicit psychoactive substances, including hallucinogens, cocaine, stimulants, and opiates. It is possible that a sample with more varied substance use profiles would yield a multiple class solution. Furthermore, the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) does not measure frequency of use across all substance classes with the same level of granularity and precision (Bucholz et al., 1994). For example, alcohol dependency is measured using SSAGA Alcohol DSM4 Dependency Diagnosis with two categories (i.e., 1 for no, 5 for yes); tobacco dependency via the SSAGA FTND Score (Fagerstrom FTND test for nicotine dependence) with seven categories (0 – 3, not dependent; 4 – 6, dependent); lifetime use of cocaine, stimulants, opiates, and sedatives with three categories (i.e., 0, never used; 1, 3 – 5 occasions and 5, more than 6 occasions); lifetime use of hallucinogens with four categories (i.e. 0, never used; 1, 1 – 2 occasions, 2, 6–10 occasions and 5, more than 10 occasion); lifetime marijuana use with six categories (i.e., 0, never used; 1, 1 – 5 occasions; 2, 6 – 10 occasions; 3, 11 – 25 occasions; 3, 26 – 50 occasions; 3, 51 – 100 occasions; 4, 101 – 999 occasions; 5, more than 1000 occasions). This may have introduced constraints on the latent structure of the substance use continuum, as well as the number of latent classes that could be identified. It will be important in future studies to use a consistent scale for measuring frequency and severity of use across substance classes, as well as to replicate the findings in an independent sample for purposes of external validation. Measurement of substance use frequency; severity and its neurobiological correlates were cross-sectional rather than longitudinal. Thus, we are unable to determine whether the properties of network connectivity of the VS, PS, and ECS reflect an underlying vulnerability to substance use or are the consequences of substance use (Ersche et al., 2010). Future work could measure functional connectivity within and between these networks in youth and determine whether network properties predict individual differences in substance use behaviour longitudinally. Lastly, the period when the SSAGA Assessment was conducted in relation to brain scan collection could have introduced some variance in the relationship between substance use and functional connectivity, since use measures may be correlated with time of year (e.g., weekend, holiday season, etc.).

5. Conclusions

In summary, we provide evidence of susceptibility biomarkers that index a continuum of substance use frequency/severity that has implications for identification of those at risk for SUDS. Furthermore, functional connectivity of decision-making systems was positively correlated with substance use severity and negatively correlated with delay discounting. The orbitofrontal, dorsolateral prefrontal, and posterior parietal cortices emerged as hubs connecting other regions of the VS, ECS and PS, possibly signalling increased valuation of multiple substance as the reward of choice. These findings could be used in combination with other clinical findings to identify individuals currently in a preaddiction state and at risk of transitioning into addiction. Prospective longitudinal studies that predict transition to SUDs based on the functional connectivity profiles identified in this study as susceptibility biomarkers would be needed to confirm our findings.

Open Article as PDF

Abstract

Substance use disorders are characterized by reduced control over the quantity and frequency of psychoactive substance use and impairments in social and occupational functioning. They are associated with poor treatment compliance and high rates of relapse. Identification of neural susceptibility biomarkers that index risk for developing a substance use disorder can facilitate earlier identification and treatment. Here, we aimed to identify the neurobiological correlates of substance use frequency and severity amongst a sample of 1,200 (652 females) participants aged 22–37 years from the Human Connectome Project. Substance use behaviour across eight classes (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, sedatives, hallucinogens, cocaine, stimulants, opiates) was measured using the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism. We explored the latent organization of substance use behaviour using a combination of exploratory structural equation modelling, latent class analysis, and factor mixture modelling to reveal a unidimensional continuum of substance use behaviour. Participants could be rank ordered along a unitary severity spectrum encompassing frequency of use of all eight substance classes, with factor score estimates generated to represent each participant’s substance use severity. Factor score estimates and delay discounting scores were compared with functional connectivity in 650 participants with imaging data using the Network-based Statistic. This neuroimaging cohort excludes participants aged 31 and over. We identified brain regions and connections correlated with impulsive decision-making and poly-substance use, with the medial orbitofrontal, lateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices emerging as key hubs. Functional connectivity of these networks could serve as susceptibility biomarkers for substance use disorders, informing earlier identification and treatment.

Background

Substance use disorders (SUDs) are conditions marked by continued use of substances despite serious negative effects. These effects include a reduced ability to control substance use, dangerous patterns of consumption, and problems in social and work life. Such disorders are common, affecting many adults. They are also linked to significant societal harms and often show poor response to treatment, with low adherence and high rates of relapse. This highlights a critical need for earlier identification and treatment. For example, the term "preaddiction" describes mild to moderate SUDs that have not yet become severe, representing a key opportunity for intervention. However, no objective biological tests currently exist to evaluate a person's risk for SUDs.

A main goal of current psychiatric research is to identify "susceptibility biomarkers." These are measurable biological traits that indicate a person's likelihood of developing a psychiatric illness. Such biomarkers hold great promise for improving mental health treatment by allowing earlier detection of problems. Over the past decade, there has been a growing interest in using findings from brain biology to improve how mental disorders are diagnosed and treated. This includes exploring shared brain mechanisms as a way to reclassify diagnostic categories, like SUDs, based on common causes, progression, treatment response, and underlying brain structures.

Traditional ways of classifying mental disorders have not been very successful in revealing the clear brain mechanisms behind these conditions. To overcome these limitations, psychiatric research is moving away from seeing mental disorders as distinct categories and towards models based on observed data and a spectrum of symptoms. These models are better at identifying shared causes of psychiatric disorders by capturing the full range of symptom severity. A large amount of evidence suggests that substance use problems exist on a continuous spectrum in the population, with no clear point where use becomes problematic. Some research also suggests that models combining aspects of both categories and dimensions, with specific subgroups within a continuous scale, might best describe certain mental health conditions.

Hybrid models, which combine features of both categorical and dimensional views of mental health, are a promising alternative for psychiatric research. One such approach is Factor Mixture Modeling (FMM). This method uses statistical analysis to identify distinct, unobservable groups or clinical types that exist within data that varies continuously. This approach can help identify clinically meaningful subgroups, which could inform earlier, more targeted interventions for individuals in a "preaddiction" state.

From a brain perspective, the proposed spectrum of substance use frequency involves several possibilities related to three key brain systems: the Valuation System (VS), the Executive Control System (ECS), and the Prospection System (PS). These systems are involved in how individuals assess value and make choices, especially when considering rewards that are available now versus later. The VS assigns value to different choices and is involved in cravings for drugs. The ECS helps inhibit impulsive reactions by considering past experiences and future goals. The PS is active when recalling memories or imagining future scenarios, including those related to drug use, and is linked to preferring rewards that are delayed.

During substance dependence, addictive substances can lead to desired states like euphoria. This process involves the activation and interaction of the VS, ECS, and PS. The pursuit of drugs can then lead to impulsive choices, where smaller, immediate rewards are preferred over larger, delayed ones. Research shows a strong link between this type of impulsivity and substance use in both adolescents and adults. Brain imaging studies have also shown altered functional connections within and between these brain networks in individuals with substance dependence. For instance, stronger connections within the fronto-parietal network (part of the ECS) and between the caudate and anterior cingulate cortex have been linked to greater impulsivity in tobacco and cocaine users. However, many of these studies focus on a single substance rather than considering the common pattern of using multiple substances.

In individuals without dependence, the VS and ECS typically maintain a balance, helping to regulate impulsive behaviors. However, in dependence, the VS's activity seems less balanced by the ECS, pushing decision-making towards drug seeking. The PS has been shown to reduce impulsive choices by counteracting the growing preference for immediate rewards (VS) and reduced cognitive control (ECS). Studies of individuals with cocaine dependence show increased activity in brain regions involved in valuation, executive control, and prospection during risky tasks, suggesting these regions are heavily involved in impulsive choices. Changes in connectivity, such as increased value placed on cocaine in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex, and weakened connections related to behavioral control, have also been observed. These findings suggest that substance dependence involves significant changes in the activation and connectivity of the VS, PS, and ECS. It is unclear, however, whether these changes are present before dependence begins and indicate a risk for developing SUDs. The brain damage caused by drugs also complicates studies seeking to identify early risk markers. Therefore, investigating neural biomarkers for SUDs in a healthy population is crucial to avoid these confounding effects.

This study proposes to investigate neural susceptibility biomarkers for SUDs in a healthy sample of young adults from the Human Connectome Project (HCP). To address variations in substance use history and the common tendency for individuals to use multiple substances, a two-stage study is proposed. The first stage involves using Factor Mixture Modeling (FMM) to characterize the patterns of substance use. It was expected that substance use across different types (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, stimulants, sedatives, and opiates) would form a single continuous spectrum. The second stage aims to identify specific brain regions and functional connections within the VS, ECS, and PS that are positively linked to this substance use spectrum among HCP participants. This analysis will use a robust network neuroscience tool called the Network-based Statistic (NBS). The expectation is that connectivity within and between the VS, ECS, and PS will be stronger as substance use and impulsive behavior increase, reflecting the role of sensation-seeking as a risk factor.

Methods

Participants and measures

A total of 1200 participants, aged 22 to 37, were included from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) Young Adult dataset. The HCP broadly defines "healthy" to include a wide range of individuals, excluding those with severe neurodevelopmental, neuropsychiatric, or neurological disorders, or conditions that could affect brain imaging data. Importantly, the dataset includes individuals who smoke, have a history of heavy drinking, or recreational drug use without severe symptoms, making it suitable for studying subclinical substance use behaviors. For this study, six participants were excluded due to incomplete data on substance use and other cognitive assessments. Key information included various self-reported measures of alcohol, nicotine, cocaine, hallucinogen, opiate, sedative, and stimulant use, as well as age and sex. These assessments were conducted before any brain scans.

Factor mixture modelling

The Factor Mixture Modeling (FMM) analysis was performed on all 1,200 participants, as larger sample sizes are beneficial for statistical techniques that identify hidden groups within data. FMM combines latent class analysis (LCA) and factor analysis. LCA identifies unobserved groups based on patterns of responses, while factor analysis explains observed responses through continuous underlying factors. FMM offers a hybrid approach, allowing for both latent classes and continuous factors, with the flexibility for factor properties to vary across classes. The analysis involved fitting various models (from restrictive to less restrictive) to the substance use data to determine the best fit. Statistical criteria were used to evaluate model performance, including measures of how well the model fit the data and how clearly it separated classes.

Delay discounting

In the HCP, delay discounting was measured for each individual using a method that calculates the "Area Under the Curve" (AUC). This approach describes an individual's tendency to devalue rewards as the time to receive them increases. The amount of a reward was adjusted until a participant was indifferent between a smaller, immediate reward and a larger, delayed one. This indifference point was then used to create a discount curve and calculate the AUC, providing a measure of impulsive choice.

Correlation between value-based decision-making network functional connectivity and substance use

Functional MRI data from the Human Connectome Project were used from 650 healthy adults. Brain regions within the Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS) were identified. For each participant, the resting-state functional connectivity (how brain regions communicate when not engaged in a specific task) was calculated between these regions across multiple scan sessions. The Network-Based Statistic (NBS) was then used to find groups of interconnected brain regions whose functional connectivity was linked to substance use patterns. This analysis also accounted for age, sex, movement during scans, delay discounting scores, cognitive abilities, socioeconomic status, and mental health diagnoses. The goal was to identify a combined subnetwork of connections that were consistently present across all four brain scan subsets, indicating robustness. A statistical threshold was applied to identify the most significant interconnected clusters of brain regions, with connections then visualized.

Results

Modelling of substance use behaviour

The analysis of substance use behavior found that a single-factor model provided the best and most straightforward representation of the data. In this model, the frequency of use for all substance types (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, hallucinogens, sedatives, opiates, and stimulants) loaded onto a single common "Substance Use" factor. This suggests that a single continuous scale effectively describes the variation in substance use across all categories within the sample. In contrast, more complex models involving two or more distinct groups, including a group characterized by very low substance use, did not fit the data as well. Despite some evidence of a high number of individuals reporting low use, a model with a single continuous dimension was superior. This finding indicated that young adults in the HCP could be ranked along a single spectrum of substance use frequency and severity. Estimates from this continuous scale were then used for further analysis with neuroimaging data.

Network-based Statistic modelling

Significant brain subnetworks were observed when the functional connectivity of regions within the VS, ECS, PS, and other specific brain areas (insula, caudate, putamen, and intraparietal sulcus) were analyzed in relation to substance use. All identified subnetworks showed a negative correlation with delay discounting, cognitive scores, and socioeconomic status (employment, education, income). Conversely, these subnetworks were positively correlated with diagnoses of depression, anxiety, somatic problems, and the substance use factor scores. Since the Network-Based Statistic analysis was performed on four separate brain scan subsets, a composite list was created, including only those connections found consistently across all four subsets.

The composite list of connections showed specific links within and between the VS, ECS, and PS. Connections within the VS included parts of the medial orbitofrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex. Connections within the ECS involved areas like the superior parietal cortex, rostral middle cingulate, rostral middle frontal, and caudal middle frontal cortices. Connections within the PS included the precuneus and superior frontal cortex. Furthermore, there were significant connections linking regions across these systems, such as VS regions connecting to ECS and PS regions, and ECS regions connecting to PS regions. These findings indicate that functional connections within and between these brain systems are associated with substance use.

Discussion

The study aimed to identify brain-based susceptibility biomarkers for Substance Use Disorders (SUDs) in young adults. The continuity hypothesis suggests that substance use exists on a spectrum from normal to problematic, and our findings support this view. Specifically, a single continuous scale of substance use frequency and severity best described the data, rather than distinct groups. This indicates that young adults in the Human Connectome Project could be ordered along a single continuum of substance use. This dimensional approach is better suited for detecting meaningful links between brain activity and behavior than categorical distinctions.

Functional connectivity within and between the VS, ECS, and PS is associated with substance use behaviour

Connections within and between the Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS) were found to be positively associated with substance use severity, as well as with depression, anxiety, and somatic problems. These connections were negatively correlated with delay discounting, a measure of impulsive choice. These results align with the initial hypotheses and can be understood in the context of reward-based decision-making and sensation-seeking, which are known risk factors for SUDs. Sensation-seeking involves a goal-directed drive to find rewarding experiences and is part of the broader concept of impulsivity, often motivating substance use. Brain regions within the ECS and PS may provide goal-directed value signals to the medial orbitofrontal cortex (part of the VS), influencing decision-making in the pursuit of addictive substances. Even in non-dependent drug users, changes in brain activation and connectivity have been observed in regions like the precuneus, superior frontal cortex (PS), posterior parietal, and lateral prefrontal cortex (ECS), which may play a role in preventing the progression to dependence. Connections between the VS and PS might contribute to thoughts about the pleasurable sensations of drug use, but numerous connections with ECS regions could help limit the impact of these thoughts and prevent a transition to dependence.

It is theorized that a delicate balance exists between brain regions involved in valuation and control, regulating impulsive behaviors. A disruption of this balance is thought to underlie the development of addictive behaviors. The current study observed a positive correlation between substance use severity and connections between regions of the VS, ECS, and PS in individuals who use multiple drugs. This suggests that while these individuals may favor mixing substances to enhance their experiences (connections between VS, ECS, PS), they might not have developed full dependence due to the protective effects of connections within and between the ECS (especially the lateral prefrontal cortex) and PS (especially the superior frontal cortex). The negative correlation between participants' delay discounting scores and network connectivity further supports the idea that the brain is working to counteract impulsivity. The more impulsive an individual is, the stronger the connections within and between the ECS and PS, possibly as a way to maintain a balance between the perceived value of drug use and top-down cognitive control.

Brain regions like the lateral prefrontal, posterior cingulate, and posterior parietal cortices emerged as key connection points within the ECS. The lateral prefrontal cortex helps direct attention and guides behavior towards rewards, sometimes disregarding risks during sensation-seeking. The posterior parietal cortex is involved in decision-making and predicts risk-taking, while the posterior cingulate cortex is thought to assign value to the substance of choice. These findings suggest that impulsive sensation-seekers willingly engage in risky, multi-drug use, possibly with an optimistic view of repeating pleasurable experiences.

Interestingly, connections were also observed between the insula and regions of the VS, ECS, and PS. The insula is believed to direct attention towards rewards and is sensitive to cues signaling preferred rewards. Increased functional connectivity was seen between the insula and parts of all three systems. These results suggest that individuals who use drugs may focus on thoughts of drugs as the primary reward, fueled by memories of past use and outcomes. This increased connectivity was negatively correlated with delay discounting scores (meaning a preference for more immediate, riskier choices), particularly through connections between the insula and the prefrontal cortex. This further suggests that reward-related attentional bias influences value-based decision-making, potentially making it harder to accept delayed rewards. Nevertheless, connections involving ECS regions may help balance the pursuit of multiple substances, possibly delaying or preventing full dependence.

The findings can be viewed in two ways. First, concerning the relationship between delay discounting and brain functional connectivity among substance users. While previous studies have shown positive correlations with delay discounting (greater impulsivity), our study found negative correlations in a sample that includes individuals with poly-substance use. This difference may reflect the complex interplay of multiple substances and increasing use severity, or the heightened value placed on combining substances for pleasure or pain relief. Individuals may crave their preferred substances and be willing to accept smaller amounts if obtained quickly, seeing multi-substance use as a fast way to achieve a desired state.

Second, regarding the practical use of these findings. Most research focuses on severe, chronic substance use, but a large portion of the population experiences mild-to-moderate SUD symptoms and accounts for more substance-related harms than those with severe SUDs. The concept of "preaddiction" highlights this state of mild-to-moderate SUDs that may precede full addiction, noting a lack of objective tools for detection. The continuous spectrum of substance use found in this study, along with the identified brain connections linked to both substance use and delay discounting, could serve as a potential neurobiological assessment for individuals in a "preaddiction" state. These models could be used alongside existing diagnostic criteria to improve assessment of patients with substance use disorder.

The study has limitations. The results of mixture modeling can sometimes be unique to specific samples, so replicating these findings in other populations is important to confirm the continuous, unidimensional spectrum of substance use and the associated brain mechanisms. The sample used was largely non-clinical, with only a small number of participants reporting illicit psychoactive substance use, which might have influenced the outcome of a single continuous spectrum rather than multiple classes. Additionally, the assessment of substance use frequency and severity across different substance types was not entirely consistent, potentially affecting the identified underlying structure. Future studies should use uniform scales and replicate findings in independent samples for external validation. The cross-sectional nature of the data means it is not possible to determine if the observed brain network properties are a cause or consequence of substance use. Future longitudinal studies tracking functional connectivity in youth could help determine if these network properties predict later substance use behavior. Lastly, the timing of substance use assessments relative to brain scan collection might have introduced some variability.

Conclusions

Evidence is presented for susceptibility biomarkers that indicate a continuous spectrum of substance use frequency and severity, which has implications for identifying individuals at risk for Substance Use Disorders (SUDs). Functional connectivity within brain systems involved in decision-making was positively associated with the severity of substance use and negatively associated with impulsive choices. Key brain regions, including the orbitofrontal, dorsolateral prefrontal, and posterior parietal cortices, emerged as central hubs connecting other regions of the Valuation, Executive Control, and Prospection Systems. This suggests an increased valuation of multiple substances as the preferred reward. These findings could be combined with other clinical information to identify individuals currently in a "preaddiction" state and at risk of progressing to addiction. Future longitudinal studies are needed to confirm these findings by predicting the transition to SUDs based on the identified functional connectivity profiles.

Open Article as PDF

Abstract

Substance use disorders are characterized by reduced control over the quantity and frequency of psychoactive substance use and impairments in social and occupational functioning. They are associated with poor treatment compliance and high rates of relapse. Identification of neural susceptibility biomarkers that index risk for developing a substance use disorder can facilitate earlier identification and treatment. Here, we aimed to identify the neurobiological correlates of substance use frequency and severity amongst a sample of 1,200 (652 females) participants aged 22–37 years from the Human Connectome Project. Substance use behaviour across eight classes (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, sedatives, hallucinogens, cocaine, stimulants, opiates) was measured using the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism. We explored the latent organization of substance use behaviour using a combination of exploratory structural equation modelling, latent class analysis, and factor mixture modelling to reveal a unidimensional continuum of substance use behaviour. Participants could be rank ordered along a unitary severity spectrum encompassing frequency of use of all eight substance classes, with factor score estimates generated to represent each participant’s substance use severity. Factor score estimates and delay discounting scores were compared with functional connectivity in 650 participants with imaging data using the Network-based Statistic. This neuroimaging cohort excludes participants aged 31 and over. We identified brain regions and connections correlated with impulsive decision-making and poly-substance use, with the medial orbitofrontal, lateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices emerging as key hubs. Functional connectivity of these networks could serve as susceptibility biomarkers for substance use disorders, informing earlier identification and treatment.

Background

Substance use disorders (SUDs) involve continued use of intoxicating substances despite serious negative effects. Symptoms include difficulty controlling use and issues with social or work functioning. These disorders are common and linked to significant societal problems, often showing poor response to treatment and high relapse rates. Thus, identifying and treating SUDs earlier is crucial. The term ‘preaddiction’ refers to less severe SUDs that may represent an important window for intervention. However, there are no objective biological tests to assess SUD risk. Modern psychiatric research aims to find biological markers that indicate a person's vulnerability to mental illness, which could lead to earlier diagnosis and improved treatment. There is growing interest in using neurobiological findings to help diagnose and treat mental disorders, including SUDs.

Traditional psychiatric research has struggled to uncover the brain mechanisms behind mental illness. To overcome this, research is moving from rigid categories of mental disorders towards models that see conditions along a spectrum, like the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). These dimensional models are better at identifying common causes of disorders by capturing variations in symptom severity. Evidence suggests that substance use problems exist on a continuous scale in the population, without a clear line between problematic and non-problematic use. Hybrid models, which combine features of both categorical and dimensional approaches, show promise. Factor mixture modeling (FMM) is one such approach. It helps identify hidden subgroups or "classes" within continuous data, even when many individuals have few or no symptoms. Such models can pinpoint clinically meaningful subtypes, which could help guide earlier, more targeted interventions for individuals in a ‘preaddiction’ state.

Neurobiologically, several brain systems are involved in how people make decisions, especially about rewards: the Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS). The VS assesses the value of choices and drives drug-seeking urges. The ECS helps inhibit impulsive responses by considering past outcomes and future goals. The PS activates when recalling memories or imagining future scenarios, including drug use, and is linked to preferring delayed rewards. In drug dependence, the interaction between these systems shifts. The VS may become dominant, leading to impulsive choices where smaller, immediate rewards (like drugs) are preferred over larger, delayed ones. While the PS can help reduce impulsive choices, the overall balance is disrupted, biasing decisions towards drug seeking. Studies using brain imaging show changes in connectivity within and between these systems in people with dependence. However, drugs themselves can cause brain damage, making it hard to know if observed brain changes are a cause or an effect of substance use. Therefore, studying these brain markers in people who are not yet dependent is crucial to avoid these confounding effects.

The present study aimed to identify neural biological markers for SUD risk in a healthy young adult group from the Human Connectome Project (HCP). To account for varied substance use patterns, a two-stage approach was proposed. First, factor mixture modeling (FMM) was used to characterize the patterns of substance use. It was expected that substance use across different types (alcohol, marijuana, other drugs) would form a single continuum, possibly with a distinct group of individuals reporting very low use. Second, the Network-based Statistic (NBS) was used to identify brain regions and functional connections within the Valuation, Executive Control, and Prospection Systems that correlate with this substance use continuum. It was hypothesized that brain connectivity within and between these systems would be stronger as substance use and impulsive behavior increased, and that higher substance use would be linked to greater impulsivity (i.e., less delay discounting).

Methods

Participants and measures

This study included 1200 participants, aged 22 to 37, drawn from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) Young Adult dataset. The HCP broadly defines "healthy" to include a diverse range of individuals, excluding those with severe mental, neurological, or physical health conditions that could affect brain imaging. However, it specifically included individuals who smoked, drank heavily, or used recreational drugs without experiencing severe symptoms. Six participants were excluded due to incomplete data. Key information gathered included self-reported substance use behaviors from the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA) covering alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, cocaine, hallucinogens, opiates, and sedatives. Participant age and sex were also included.

Factor mixture modelling

Factor mixture modeling (FMM) was used to analyze substance use data from all 1200 participants. FMM is a statistical technique that combines two approaches: factor analysis and latent class analysis. Factor analysis identifies a continuous underlying dimension, or "factor," that explains variations in observed behaviors. Latent class analysis, on the other hand, identifies distinct, unobserved subgroups, or "classes," within a population. FMM is a hybrid method that can identify both continuous dimensions and discrete classes at the same time, allowing for a more flexible understanding of complex behaviors like substance use. This approach can also account for situations where many individuals report very low or no symptoms (known as "zero-inflation"). Different versions of FMM were tested. The goal was to find the model that best fit the data, determining whether substance use was best described as a single continuum, multiple distinct classes, or a combination of both. Statistical criteria were used to compare these different models and select the most appropriate one for understanding the patterns of substance use among the participants.

Delay discounting

Delay discounting, a measure of impulsivity (preferring immediate, smaller rewards over delayed, larger ones), was calculated for each participant. The Human Connectome Project used a method called Area Under the Curve (AUC), which reflects an individual's overall tendency to discount future rewards.

Correlation between value-based decision-making network functional connectivity and substance use

Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from 650 participants were analyzed. Brain regions related to the Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS) were identified. Functional connectivity, or how different brain regions communicate, was measured by calculating correlations between the activity signals of these regions during rest. The Network-Based Statistic (NBS) was then used to identify networks of brain connections that were statistically related to the substance use continuum scores. Other factors such as age, sex, impulsivity levels, cognitive abilities, socioeconomic status, and mental health diagnoses (depression, anxiety, somatic problems) were also included in the analysis to account for their influence. A single, combined network was then created using connections found consistently across all four fMRI scans for each participant. This helped ensure the findings were robust.

Results

Modelling of substance use behaviour

Analysis of substance use behavior showed that a single, continuous dimension best represented the data. This means that participants' reported use of various substances (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, stimulants, sedatives, opiates, cocaine) could be placed along one consistent scale, from low to high frequency of use. While some individuals reported very low or no substance use, statistical models attempting to identify distinct subgroups (like a "zero-inflated" class) within this continuum did not fit the data well. Instead, a single-factor model proved to be the most accurate. Factor scores, representing each participant's position on this substance use continuum, were then used for further brain imaging analysis. Regarding demographic factors, males and older participants tended to have higher substance use scores. Additionally, individuals whose mothers or fathers had a history of substance use also reported higher substance use themselves.

Network-based Statistic modelling

Significant brain networks were identified where functional connectivity was related to substance use behavior. These networks involved regions within and connected to the Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS), as well as other key brain areas like the insula. The strength of connections within these networks was positively correlated with higher substance use and with mental health diagnoses such as depression, anxiety, and somatic problems. Conversely, these connections were negatively correlated with delay discounting (a measure of impulsivity), cognitive abilities, and socioeconomic factors like employment, education, and income. A combined network of connections consistently found across all brain scans was created, highlighting key functional pathways associated with substance use in this population.

Discussion

The study's findings support the idea that substance use behavior exists along a single, continuous spectrum in young adults, rather than being divided into distinct categories. This means that a person's use of various substances can be placed on a single scale from low to high frequency. While some individuals reported minimal or no substance use, models attempting to identify separate subgroups did not fit the data as well as a single continuous dimension. This highlights the value of using a dimensional approach to study substance use, as it can better reveal meaningful connections between brain activity and behavior.

Functional connectivity within and between the VS, ECS, and PS is associated with substance use behaviour

The findings indicate that connections within and between the brain's Valuation, Executive Control, and Prospection Systems are positively linked to increased substance use, as well as symptoms of depression, anxiety, and somatic problems. These connections were also negatively correlated with delay discounting, which means that stronger connections were associated with less impulsive choices. These results align with the idea that sensation-seeking—a tendency to pursue rewarding experiences—is a factor in substance use. Even among non-dependent users, the Executive Control and Prospection Systems appear to play a role in preventing the progression to full dependence, possibly by providing top-down control that restricts the impact of drug-related thoughts. Specific brain regions, such as the lateral prefrontal, posterior cingulate, and posterior parietal cortices, emerged as key communication hubs. These areas are involved in directing attention towards rewards, making decisions, and assessing the value of substances. The insula, a region involved in directing attention towards rewards and responding to cues, also showed increased connectivity with these systems. This suggests a strong focus on drugs as preferred rewards, potentially making it harder to choose delayed, non-drug rewards. However, the involvement of Executive Control System regions may help balance the desire for poly-substance pursuit, potentially preventing the onset of dependence. While some previous studies found different correlations with impulsivity, the current findings may reflect the complexities of poly-substance use and the increased value placed on combining substances to achieve desired effects. These findings are important because they offer potential biological markers for individuals in a 'preaddiction' state—those with mild to moderate substance use disorders who may be at risk for developing more severe addiction. Such objective assessments could be used alongside existing diagnostic tools to identify individuals who could benefit from early intervention.

Limitations

This study has several limitations. The findings regarding the continuous nature of substance use should be replicated in other groups, as statistical models can sometimes be specific to the sample studied. The current sample was largely non-clinical, with limited reports of illicit substance use, which might have influenced the observed single-continuum pattern. Additionally, the assessment tool for substance use (SSAGA) used different levels of detail for various substances, potentially affecting the precision of the substance use continuum. Future research should use consistent measures and be replicated in independent samples for validation. As this was a single-point-in-time study, it cannot determine whether the observed brain network properties cause substance use vulnerability or are a consequence of substance use. Longitudinal studies that track brain changes over time would be needed to clarify this. Finally, the timing of substance use assessments relative to brain scans could have introduced some variability.

Conclusions

In summary, this study provides evidence of biological markers in the brain that are linked to a continuous spectrum of substance use severity. Functional connections within and between the brain's decision-making systems (Valuation, Executive Control, and Prospection) were positively associated with more severe substance use and negatively associated with impulsivity. Specific brain regions, including parts of the orbitofrontal, dorsolateral prefrontal, and posterior parietal cortices, acted as key connection points, suggesting an increased value placed on multiple substances. These findings could potentially be combined with other clinical information to identify individuals in a 'preaddiction' state, who are at higher risk of developing severe substance use disorders. Future long-term studies are needed to confirm if these brain connectivity patterns can predict the development of SUDs over time.

Open Article as PDF

Abstract

Substance use disorders are characterized by reduced control over the quantity and frequency of psychoactive substance use and impairments in social and occupational functioning. They are associated with poor treatment compliance and high rates of relapse. Identification of neural susceptibility biomarkers that index risk for developing a substance use disorder can facilitate earlier identification and treatment. Here, we aimed to identify the neurobiological correlates of substance use frequency and severity amongst a sample of 1,200 (652 females) participants aged 22–37 years from the Human Connectome Project. Substance use behaviour across eight classes (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, sedatives, hallucinogens, cocaine, stimulants, opiates) was measured using the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism. We explored the latent organization of substance use behaviour using a combination of exploratory structural equation modelling, latent class analysis, and factor mixture modelling to reveal a unidimensional continuum of substance use behaviour. Participants could be rank ordered along a unitary severity spectrum encompassing frequency of use of all eight substance classes, with factor score estimates generated to represent each participant’s substance use severity. Factor score estimates and delay discounting scores were compared with functional connectivity in 650 participants with imaging data using the Network-based Statistic. This neuroimaging cohort excludes participants aged 31 and over. We identified brain regions and connections correlated with impulsive decision-making and poly-substance use, with the medial orbitofrontal, lateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices emerging as key hubs. Functional connectivity of these networks could serve as susceptibility biomarkers for substance use disorders, informing earlier identification and treatment.

Background

Substance use disorders (SUDs) involve ongoing use of substances despite significant harmful effects. Common signs include losing control over how much or how often a substance is used, engaging in risky consumption, and experiencing problems at work or in social settings. These disorders are quite common, affecting many adults. There is a clear need to identify and treat SUDs earlier. The term 'preaddiction' refers to milder forms of SUDs that have not yet become severe, representing a crucial time for intervention. However, there are currently no biological tests to assess a person's risk for developing a SUD.

A main goal of modern mental health research is to find biological markers—measurable characteristics in the body—that indicate a person's vulnerability to mental illness. These markers hold great promise for improving treatment by allowing earlier detection of problems. Over the past decade, there has been a growing interest in using findings from brain science to better diagnose and treat mental disorders. This includes looking for shared brain biology across different conditions, which could help reorganize diagnostic categories for disorders like SUDs.

Traditional psychiatric research has struggled to uncover clear brain mechanisms behind mental health problems. To overcome this, the field is moving away from simply categorizing mental disorders as distinct conditions. Instead, it is adopting models that view conditions like substance use on a continuous scale, such as the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). These "dimensional" models are better at identifying common causes of disorders by capturing the full range of symptom severity. Evidence shows that substance use problems exist along a continuum in the population, without a clear dividing line between non-problematic and problematic use. Hybrid models, which combine features of both categorical and dimensional approaches, show promise for psychiatric research. A statistical technique called Factor Mixture Modeling (FMM) can identify distinct, hidden groups within continuous data. This approach can help identify important subgroups, such as those in a 'preaddiction' state, allowing for earlier, more targeted interventions.

From a brain perspective, the patterns of substance use may reflect changes in specific brain systems involved in decision-making and valuing rewards. These include the Valuation System (VS), which processes the value of choices and generates cravings; the Executive Control System (ECS), which helps inhibit impulsive actions; and the Prospection System (PS), which is involved in imagining future scenarios, including substance use, and is linked to preferring rewards later rather than immediately. These systems work together during decision-making.

During substance dependence, the pursuit of drugs can lead to "choice impulsivity," a tendency to prefer smaller, immediate rewards over larger, delayed ones. Research shows a strong link between this impulsivity and substance use. Normally, the VS and ECS maintain a balance to regulate impulsive behaviors. However, in dependence, the VS's influence becomes stronger, biasing decisions towards drug use. The PS appears to help reduce impulsive choices by counteracting this imbalance. Studies of individuals with cocaine or alcohol dependence show significant changes in the connections within and between these brain systems, indicating that their decision-making is heavily skewed towards drugs, even despite negative consequences. It is crucial to study these potential brain markers for SUDs in people who have not yet become dependent, to avoid confusing changes caused by drug use with underlying vulnerabilities.

Methods

This study used data from 1,200 young adult participants, aged 22 to 37, gathered as part of the Human Connectome Project (HCP). The HCP aimed to create a broad sample of healthy individuals, representing a wide range of backgrounds. It included people who smoked, had a history of heavy drinking, or used recreational drugs, as long as they had not experienced severe symptoms of a disorder. Researchers collected information on various types of substance use (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, stimulants, sedatives, opiates, cocaine), as well as age and sex, using standardized assessments.

To understand the patterns of substance use behavior, Factor Mixture Modeling (FMM) was applied to the data. This statistical method combines approaches that identify distinct groups (like categories) with those that measure differences along a continuous scale. The goal was to see if substance use was best described as a single spectrum of frequency and severity, or if there were hidden, distinct groups of users within the sample.

In addition, the study measured "delay discounting" for each participant, which indicates a person's tendency to choose smaller, immediate rewards over larger, delayed ones. Brain imaging data (functional magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI) from 1,008 HCP participants was also used. Researchers focused on connections within and between the brain's Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS). They used a technique called Network-Based Statistic (NBS) to identify specific networks of brain connections that were linked to the substance use patterns identified by FMM, as well as to delay discounting scores, cognitive abilities, socioeconomic status, and mental health conditions like depression and anxiety.

Results

The analysis of substance use behavior revealed that a single, continuous scale best represented the data. This means that individuals in the study could be ranked along a spectrum of substance use frequency and severity across all substance types (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, hallucinogens, sedatives, opiates, and stimulants), rather than falling into distinct categories of users. While there were some signs of very low use among many participants, a model showing a single continuous dimension of substance use provided the best fit for the data. The study also found that males and older participants, as well as those with a history of maternal or paternal substance use, tended to have higher substance use scores, though these effects were generally small.

When examining brain connectivity, significant networks of connections were found within and between the Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS). These brain connections were positively linked to higher substance use levels and also to diagnoses of depression, anxiety, and somatic problems. Conversely, these connections were negatively linked to delay discounting (meaning stronger connections were associated with less impulsive choices). These findings suggest that specific patterns of brain connectivity are associated with substance use behavior in young adults.

Discussion

This study confirmed that substance use behavior can be best understood as a single, continuous spectrum of frequency and severity, rather than as separate categories. This dimensional approach proved effective in revealing meaningful connections between brain activity and substance use patterns.

Stronger connections within and between the brain's Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS) were linked to higher substance use, as well as to mental health conditions like depression and anxiety. These stronger connections were also associated with less impulsivity (lower delay discounting), which aligns with the idea that sensation-seeking, a facet of impulsivity, can be a predisposing factor for substance use disorders. Specific brain regions, including the lateral prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate, posterior parietal cortices, and the insula, emerged as key connection points within these networks. The involvement of these regions suggests that individuals might become highly focused on drugs as rewards, a focus potentially fueled by memories of past use and an optimistic outlook on future use. However, the numerous connections with ECS regions, especially the lateral prefrontal cortex, could indicate a protective effect, potentially helping to limit the impact of drug-related thoughts and prevent the transition to full dependence.

These findings suggest a potential objective measure for identifying individuals in a "preaddiction" state, which refers to mild-to-moderate substance use disorders that occur before severe addiction develops. The specific brain connection patterns identified in this study, when combined with other clinical assessments, could serve as biological markers for early risk detection. Such markers could help clinicians intervene sooner and more effectively.

Despite these insights, the study has limitations. The findings might be specific to this particular group of participants and require confirmation in other populations, particularly those with more varied substance use profiles. Since the study collected data at a single point in time, it cannot determine whether the observed brain network properties are underlying vulnerabilities to substance use or a consequence of it. Future longitudinal studies that follow individuals over time will be essential to confirm if these brain connectivity patterns predict the later development of substance use disorders.

Conclusions

The study provides evidence of potential biological markers that indicate a continuum of substance use frequency and severity, which has implications for identifying individuals at risk for SUDs. Functional connectivity within brain systems involved in decision-making was positively correlated with substance use severity and negatively correlated with impulsive choices. Key brain regions, including the orbitofrontal, dorsolateral prefrontal, and posterior parietal cortices, emerged as important connection points, possibly signaling an increased valuation of multiple substances as preferred rewards. These findings could be used in conjunction with other clinical information to help identify individuals who are in a "preaddiction" state and are at risk of progressing to addiction. Future long-term studies are necessary to confirm if the identified functional connectivity profiles truly predict the development of SUDs.

Open Article as PDF

Abstract

Substance use disorders are characterized by reduced control over the quantity and frequency of psychoactive substance use and impairments in social and occupational functioning. They are associated with poor treatment compliance and high rates of relapse. Identification of neural susceptibility biomarkers that index risk for developing a substance use disorder can facilitate earlier identification and treatment. Here, we aimed to identify the neurobiological correlates of substance use frequency and severity amongst a sample of 1,200 (652 females) participants aged 22–37 years from the Human Connectome Project. Substance use behaviour across eight classes (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, sedatives, hallucinogens, cocaine, stimulants, opiates) was measured using the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism. We explored the latent organization of substance use behaviour using a combination of exploratory structural equation modelling, latent class analysis, and factor mixture modelling to reveal a unidimensional continuum of substance use behaviour. Participants could be rank ordered along a unitary severity spectrum encompassing frequency of use of all eight substance classes, with factor score estimates generated to represent each participant’s substance use severity. Factor score estimates and delay discounting scores were compared with functional connectivity in 650 participants with imaging data using the Network-based Statistic. This neuroimaging cohort excludes participants aged 31 and over. We identified brain regions and connections correlated with impulsive decision-making and poly-substance use, with the medial orbitofrontal, lateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices emerging as key hubs. Functional connectivity of these networks could serve as susceptibility biomarkers for substance use disorders, informing earlier identification and treatment.

Background

Substance use disorders (SUDs) mean using drugs or alcohol even when it causes big problems. These problems can be using too much, or having trouble at work or home. Finding SUDs early is important. Sometimes, people have milder problems called "preaddiction," which is a good time to get help. Researchers need better ways to test for SUD risk using parts of the body, like the brain.

Scientists are looking for "biomarkers." These are measurable body signs that show if someone is likely to get a mental illness. Biomarkers can help doctors find problems sooner. Researchers are especially looking at how brain activity might be shared across different mental health issues, including SUDs.

Older ways of studying mental health did not fully show how brain problems cause these conditions. So, new research now looks at mental health problems more like a continuous scale, not just as a "yes" or "no" illness. For example, how much someone uses drugs is seen as a line, from not using at all to using a lot. New study methods, like "factor mixture modeling," help find if there are different hidden groups of people within this long scale of drug use.

When people make choices, especially about rewards, specific parts of the brain work together. The Valuation System (VS) helps decide how much something is worth. The Executive Control System (ECS) helps control quick, unplanned actions. The Prospection System (PS) helps think about future events. These brain systems are active when people make decisions about things like using drugs.

With drug dependence, these brain systems can change. People might choose small, quick rewards over larger, delayed ones, which is called "impulsive choice." Past studies show a link between this impulsive choice and drug use, with stronger brain connections in certain areas for people who are more impulsive. It is important to study these brain changes in healthy people before severe dependence to avoid confusion from drug effects on the brain.

How the Study Was Done

This study looked at 1200 young adults (ages 22 to 37) from a large brain study called the Human Connectome Project (HCP). These people were chosen to be generally healthy and represent many different backgrounds. The study collected information about their drug and alcohol use, and how they made choices about rewards (called "delay discounting").

Researchers used a special math method called "factor mixture modeling" to understand patterns in the drug use data. They looked to see if drug use was a single line (like a scale from low to high) or if there were different groups of users. They also used brain scans from 1008 of these people to look at how different brain parts connect.

Brain Connections and Substance Use

The study focused on connections within and between the Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS) in the brain. Researchers used special brain imaging (fMRI) to see how these parts connected. They looked for links between these brain connections and a person's level of substance use, as well as their impulsive choices, age, sex, and other health information. They looked for connections that were seen across all different brain scans to make sure the findings were strong.

Substance Use Patterns

The study found that all the different types of substance use (alcohol, tobacco, marijuana, and other drugs) fit together on one single scale. This means that people's substance use could be seen as a continuous line, from very low use to higher use. The study did not find clear, separate groups of users. This result showed that a single "Substance Use" score could be given to each person to show where they fell on this scale.

Brain Network Findings

The study then looked at how this "Substance Use" score was linked to brain connections. It found important connections within the Valuation, Executive Control, and Prospection systems of the brain. These connections were stronger in people with higher substance use scores. Also, these stronger connections were linked to more impulsive choices, and to having feelings of depression or anxiety.

Discussion

This study aimed to find signs in the brain that show risk for substance use disorders (SUDs) in young adults. The findings suggest that substance use is a single, continuous scale, not divided into separate groups. This means people can be ranked from low to high substance use. This idea of a continuous scale helps researchers understand how the brain might be linked to substance use.

The study found that connections within and between the Valuation System (VS), Executive Control System (ECS), and Prospection System (PS) in the brain were stronger in people who used more substances. These stronger connections were also linked to feeling depressed or anxious. This might mean these brain areas are working harder to control impulsive behaviors, especially when people use multiple substances.

These brain findings can be understood through the idea of "sensation-seeking," where people want exciting experiences, which can lead to using more substances. The ECS and PS brain areas seem to play a role in helping control these risky behaviors. For example, connections involving the prefrontal cortex and superior frontal cortex might help stop a person from becoming fully dependent, even if they use multiple drugs.

Another important finding was how the "insula" brain area connected to the other systems. The insula is involved in focusing attention on rewards. Stronger connections between the insula and other brain parts were seen in people who were more likely to seek out drugs as a reward. This suggests that the brain might be heavily focused on drugs, making it harder to choose rewards that come later.

This research is important because most studies look at severe addiction. But many people have milder substance use problems, or "preaddiction," and these problems also cause a lot of harm. The brain patterns found in this study could be a way to objectively test for risk in people who are in this "preaddiction" stage. However, the study has some limits. The people in this study were generally healthy, and the way substance use was measured was not the same for all drugs. Also, this study looked at a single point in time, so it cannot say if these brain changes cause substance use or are a result of it. Future studies need to look at these things over time.

Conclusions

This study found brain signs that show a continuous scale of substance use. It also showed that the way certain brain systems (VS, ECS, PS) connect is linked to how much someone uses substances and how impulsively they make choices. These findings could help find people at risk for substance use disorders, especially those in a "preaddiction" stage. More studies are needed to see if these brain connections can predict future addiction.

Open Article as PDF

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Loganathan, K., & Tiego, J. (2023). Value-based decision-making network functional connectivity correlates with substance use and delay discounting behaviour among young adults. NeuroImage. Clinical, 38, 103424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2023.103424

    Highlights