The association between delinquent peer affiliation and disruptive behavior interacts with functional brain correlates of reward sensitivity:A biosocial interaction study in adolescent delinquents
Jorim J. Tielbeek
Maarten van der Hooft
Moran D. Cohn
Peter M. van de Ven
Tinca J.C. Polderman
SimpleOriginal

Summary

This study shows for the first time how brain reward sensitivity (VS) interacts with spending time with delinquent peers (DPA) to influence the severity of disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) in adolescents.

2024

The association between delinquent peer affiliation and disruptive behavior interacts with functional brain correlates of reward sensitivity:A biosocial interaction study in adolescent delinquents

Keywords biosocial criminology; childhood arrestees; conduct disorder; delinquent peer affiliation; disruptive behavioral disorder; fMRI; reward sensitivity; ventral striatum; amygdala; mPFC; DISC; youth delinquents

Abstract

Affiliating with delinquent peers may stimulate the development of antisocial behavior, especially for adolescents who are sensitive to social rewards. The current study examines whether the association between delinquent peer affiliation (DPA) and disruptive behavior interacts with functional brain correlates of reward sensitivity in early onset male adolescents delinquents. Childhood arrestees (n = 126, mean age = 17.7 [S.D. 1.6]) completed a DPA questionnaire, and participated in an fMRI study in which reward sensitivity was operationalized through responsiveness of the ventral striatum (VS), amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during the monetary incentive delay paradigm (reward anticipation and outcome). Symptoms of disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) were assessed through structured psychiatric interviews (Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children) with adolescents. DPA had a main effect on DBD symptoms. Adolescents with high VS reward responses showed a stronger significant positive association between DPA and DBD symptoms compared to low VS responders. No evidence for an interaction effect was found for the amygdala and mPFC. Post-hoc analyses revealed the positive association between DPA and DBD was only present in males, with a diminishing effect as age increased. We found evidence for a biosocial interaction between DPA and reward sensitivity of the VS in relation to DBD symptom severity. This study provides the first evidence of an interaction effect between a brain mechanism and an environmental factor in relation to DBD symptoms, implying that susceptibility to influences of delinquent peers may intertwine with individual biological differences.

Introduction

Adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) show increased antisocial and aggressive behavior, emotional dysregulation, and risky decision-making. DBD is associated with high societal impact and public health costs, due to their aggregate burden on the perpetrators themselves, victims, their families, and communities (Foster & Jones, 2005; Rivenbark et al., 2018). Despite the significant impairments associated with DBD, the underlying mechanisms of antisocial development are only partly understood, necessitating further research to improve the effectiveness of prevention and intervention strategies (Epstein, Fonnesbeck, Potter, Rizzone, & McPheeters, 2015; Eyberg, Nelson, & Boggs, 2008). A wide range of family and twin studies have shown that about half of the variance in antisocial behavior can be explained by environmental factors, whereas the other half is explained by genetic influences (Polderman et al., 2015). Over the past few decades, the majority of research designs addressing the etiology of ASB have included explanatory models integrating either environmental factors or biological factors. In the present study, we test for the presence of an interaction effect by examining brain correlates of reward sensitivity and delinquent peer affiliation (DPA) in relation to DBD symptom scores.

Delinquent peer affiliation and DBD

An important environmental risk factor associated with DBD is the exposure to delinquent peers. For instance, a longitudinal study of Simonoff et al. (2004) demonstrated that adolescents with antisocial peers are five times more likely to commit a crime during adolescence and ten times more likely to be diagnosed with antisocial personality disorder in adulthood than adolescents without delinquent peers (Simonoff et al., 2004). Despite the well-supported association between DPA and DBD, the nature of this relationship has been debated. Scholars have proposed two general mechanisms explaining the association: (A) causal socializing effects of DPA on DBD, such that affiliating with deviant peers leads to more engagement in disruptive behavior or (B) non-causal selection processes, such that disruptive individuals choose to affiliate with disruptive peers (Fergusson, Woodward, & Horwood, 1999; Moffitt, 1993). Fergusson, Swain-Campbell, and Horwood (2002) found support for both processes being complementary and reported that after controlling for selection, the strength of the association between DPA and disruptive outcome measures was reduced, yet still present (Fergusson et al., 2002). The relative influence of these processes has been shown to vary over age, with stronger selection effects in middle adolescence, and more powerful socialization effects in late adolescence (Monahan, Steinberg, & Cauffman, 2009). Although individuals with deviant peers are more likely to engage in antisocial behavior, not all individuals are equally influenced by their friends. A number of studies have explored which factors underlie these individual differences in vulnerability to peer influences. For example, the relationship between DPA and DBD in adolescents was found to depend on the presence of callous and unemotional traits (CU-traits), such that DBD adolescents with CU-traits displayed higher levels of DPA (Kimonis, Frick, & Barry, 2004). In contrast, another study found that high prosocial involvement was a protective factor against the contagion effects of DPA (Kaufmann, Wyman, Forbes-Jones, & Barry, 2007). The individual variability in susceptibility to environmental influences may in turn be explained by the level of resistance to peer influences. Monahan et al. (2009) found that socialization effects on selfreported antisocial behavior were moderated by a set of personality characteristics referred to as peer influence susceptibility, such that socialization processes were stronger among adolescents with low resistance compared to those with a high resistance to peer influence (Monahan et al., 2009). Moreover, studies examining general peer-relationship processes have also demonstrated sexdifferences in how peers contribute to emotional and behavioral development. In their review, (Rose & Rudolph, 2006) found that female-linked peer relationships generally inhibit antisocial behavior, whereas male-linked relationships generally contribute to behavioral problems.

Adolescence and reward processing

Adolescence is characterized by increased social interactions, risk taking, impulsivity, and reward sensitivity (Casey, Jones, & Hare, 2008). This crucial transitional developmental period is also marked by drastic neurobiological and hormonal changes. Preclinical studies have shown that various components of the dopamine system go through extensive structural changes during adolescence, i.e. there is a peak in dopamine transporter and D1-like receptor density in the rat striatum (Moll et al., 2000; Tarazi, Tomasini, & Baldessarini, 1999), and ventral tegmental area (VTA) dopamine neurons in adolescent rats fire faster than in adult rats (McCutcheon et al., 2012). Neuroimaging work strongly supports the notion that the greater reward-seeking behavior during adolescence is reflected by a hyper-responsive reward system in the brain (Galvan, 2010). In addition, developmental cognitive neuroscience studies have shown adolescentspecific influences on reward system function, with elevated recruitment of subcortical limbic systems relative to top-down (cortical) control systems during adolescence compared to childhood and adulthood (Galvan et al., 2006; Steinberg et al., 2008). Casey, Jones, and Somerville (2011) have proposed a neurobiological model of adolescent development describing the imbalance between an accelerated responsiveness to motivational cues and immaturity in cognitive control (Casey et al., 2011).

These findings suggest that structural and functional changes in corticosubcortical circuitry during this developmental period may predispose adolescents to a higher sensitivity to reward-related cues, such as cooperative peer-interaction.

Susceptibility to peer influence, brain function, and risk taking behavior

Neuroimaging studies have shown activation of the mesolimbic dopamine reward system in response to peer-interactions, particularly cooperative and socially shared context as well as emotional (positive and negative) experiences recruit the VS (Schilbach et al., 2010; Tabibnia & Lieberman, 2007; Wagner et al., 2015). Likewise, other human brain imaging studies revealed individual differences in dopaminergic neurotransmission in reward-related circuits, which may lead to different responses to environmental cues, such as peer influences. Chein and others (2011) demonstrated that, in the awareness of their peers, adolescents showed an amplified activity in reward-related brain regions, including the VS, which had a subsequent riskpromoting effect (Chein, Albert, O’Brien, Uckert, & Steinberg, 2011). The adolescent brain’s incentive processing system would thus differentially respond to the potential reward of risky choices, dependent on the presence or absence of peers.

Neuroimaging studies have also reported significant group differences between antisocial adolescents and healthy controls concerning reward-sensitivity (Cohn et al., 2015; Gatzke-Kopp et al., 2009; Matthys, Vanderschuren, Schutter, & Lochman, 2012). Buckholtz and others demonstrated that individuals displaying impulsive-antisocial behavior show heightened dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens during monetary reinforcement, compared to healthy controls (Buckholtz et al., 2010). In addition, a multi-modality functional imaging study found that maladaptive decision-making in incarcerated offenders was associated with a disrupted regulation in the cortico-striatal circuit (Hosking et al., 2017). Neuropsychological and physiological studies have shed light on the amygdala’s multifaceted role in reward processing, expanding beyond its well-known association with fear conditioning and the processing of negative emotions (Morrison & Salzman, 2010). The amygdala has been shown to play a key role in learning about the positive value of stimuli, demonstrating its involvement in reward representation and stimulus-reward learning (Baxter & Murray, 2002). Lastly, extensive preclinical and human research emphasizes the critical role of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in incentive processing (Tzschentke, 2000; Xue et al., 2009). Notably, previous fMRI studies have implicated the mPFC as a pivotal region mediating the effects of stress on reward sensitivity (Ironside, Kumar, Kang, & Pizzagalli, 2018; Treadway, Buckholtz, & Zald, 2013). Furthermore, the mPFC is instrumental in integrating sensory information with learned emotional values and exerting cortical control over reward-based behavioral output (Pastor & Medina, 2021).

In light of these findings, the present study explored whether the strength of the association between DPA and DBD could be linked to the responsiveness of reward-circuitries. Neuroimaging research has identified the ventral striatum, amygdala, and mPFC as crucial brain areas involved in reward processing (Baxter & Murray, 2002; Haber, 2011; Murray, 2007; O’Doherty, 2004). To assess sensitivity to reward, neural activity in the VS, amygdala, and mPFC during the fMRI-based monetary incentive delay (MID) task, which involves reward anticipation and outcome response, was employed as an intermediate phenotype. While the MID paradigm serves as a well-established and controlled measure of reward sensitivity, it is crucial to acknowledge that social rewards may entail more intricate and nuanced processes that extend beyond simple monetary incentives. Prior research has demonstrated that tangible and quantitative social rewards hold greater incentive power among children and adolescents compared to monetary rewards (Wang, Liu, & Shi, 2017). Interestingly, although studies have shown that the neural basis for anticipating social approval is similar to that of anticipating monetary rewards, men exhibit a wider network of mesolimbic brain regions activated during the prospect of monetary rewards, whereas limited activation is observed for social rewards (Spreckelmeyer et al., 2009). We hypothesized that increased reward-sensitivity is related to DBD symptoms, particularly in the context of DPA.

Materials and methods

Subjects

The present study makes use of phenotypic and neuroimaging data collected during May 2011 – June 2012 in a subsample of childhood onset offenders at the mean age of 17.7 (S.D. 1.6) years, of which 85% were males (Cohn et al., 2015). Subjects were drawn from a cohort of 364 first-time arrestees recruited in three different police regions in the Netherlands during the period of July 2003 – December 2005 (van Domburgh, Vermeiren, Blokland, & Doreleijers, 2009). The participants at that time were all younger than 12 years, which is the age of criminal responsibility for Dutch law. This subsample was selected to represent the entire severity spectrum of antisocial behavior, ranging from low-risk to high-risk, based on previous waves’ DBD symptom counts (NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule, DISC-IV (Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab-Stone, 2000)), aggression scores (Reactive Proactive Aggression Questionnaire, RPQ (Raine et al., 2006); see below) and psychopathic traits scores (The Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory, YPI (Andershed, Hodgins, & Tengström, 2007)). We employed the same exclusion criteria as Cohn et al. (2015) and added missing DPA data as an extra criterion, resulting in the exclusion of 24 out of the 150 participants on the basis of invalid or missing MRI data (n = 10), missing DPA data (n = 2), task performance rates on the MID task deviating more than 3 S.D. from the mean (n = 9) or drug use in the last 24 h before scanning (n = 3). The excluded group (n = 24) and the study sample (n = 126) were similar regarding levels of antisocial behavior, aggression, psychopathic traits, age, IQ, sex, ethnicity, or socioeconomic status. Comparing the neuroimaging subsample to the remainder of the childhood arrestees cohort, we found that the study sample had higher initial DBD symptom scores. For a complete description of the recruitment strategy, see Cohn et al. (2015).

Measurements and procedure

Delinquent peer affiliation

DPA was assessed by a self-report questionnaire developed by the RADAR study (Research on Adolescent Development and Relationships; (37)). This questionnaire consists of eleven items, scored on a three-point scale, of which the first half measured the peer delinquency, whereas the second half measured the level of peer affiliation. The first six items were summed to index ‘crime level peers’ and were scored using a three-choice response format (0 = none of my friends are like that, 1 = just a few of my friends are like that, 2 = most of my friends are like that). The last five items were summed in the same way to index peer affiliation. The DPA score (ranging from 0 to 120) was displayed by multiplying crime level peers score with the peer affiliation score. At baseline interview, the internal consistency of the two subscales was adequate (peer delinquency, Cronbachs α = 0.83; peer affiliation, α = 0.74).

Disruptive behavior disorder symptoms

The VU University Medical Ethics Committee and the Ministry of Justice approved the phenotypic and neuroimaging data collection. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants and from their parents or guardians (in case participants were below 18 years of age). Behavioral data on DBD were collected retrospectively using a structured psychiatric interview (DISC-IV, child and parent version) (Knutson, Adams, Fong, & Hommer, 2001a), administered at home. The DISC-IV assessment evaluates DBD symptomatology within the past twelve months. Therefore, DBD symptoms related to their initial offense, which may have led to their arrest, were not included in their DBD-score at the age of 18. DBD-score was computed as a continuous measure based on whether symptoms were scored for CD and ODD on the child version of the DISC.

MID task

We used the MID paradigm as described by Cohn and others that consisted of 72 six second trials (Knutson, Fong, Adams, Varner, & Hommer, 2001b). During the MID-task subjects anticipated on a potential monetary reward, punishment or no consequences depending on the type of cue. Cues signaling the potential of winning €0.50 were denoted by circles, the potential of losing €0.50 were denoted by squares and no monetary outcome was denoted by triangles. Within each trial, participants saw one of three cue shapes (cue, 2000 msec), fixated on a crosshair as they waited during a variable interval (delay, 2000–2500 msec), and then responded to a white target square that appeared for a variable length of time (target 160–260 msec) with a button press. After target presentation, the outcome (outcome, 1650 msec), which followed the disappearance of the target, notified participants whether they had won or lost money during that trial and indicated their cumulative total at that point. The MID paradigm has been tested extensively and successfully recruits the reward regions of the brain, which is also reflected in the expected activation in the limbic and frontostriatal neurocircuitry in the present study (Cohn et al., 2015; Knutson et al., 2001a; Knutson et al., 2001b).

Our study focused on reward sensitivity by observing the reward trials. The outcome of these reward trials could be either (1) ‘reward outcome’ if the participant pressed the button fast enough during target presentation or (2) ‘no reward outcome’ if the participant was too slow. Trial types were pseudo randomly ordered within each session. Task difficulty, based on reaction times collected during the practice session before scanning, was set such that each participant should succeed on 66% of his or her target responses (Knutson et al., 2001a).

fMRI acquisition

Functional MRI data were obtained on a second occasion, during which participants were scanned in a Philips 3 T Intera MRI-scanner in the Academic Medical Center Amsterdam. Neuroimaging generally took place within 1.5 months after the behavioral session, with a few exceptions where the scan was performed after 2 months. During this scan protocol, T1 weighted anatomical scans were acquired using a 8-channel SENSE headcoil and consisted of 180 sagittal 1 mm thickness slices, with an in-plane resolution of 1 × 1 mm (FOV 256X256X180 mm, TR 9.0 ms, TE 3.5 ms). Furthermore, 204 T2* weighted echo planar images (EPI) were acquired, each volume consisting of 38 ascending slices of 3 mm thickness and 2.29 × 2.29 in-plane resolution, parallel to the anterior commissure – posterior commissure line (FOV 220 × 220 mm, TR 2300, TE 30 ms).

fMRI analysis

Functional MRI data were processed using SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK) in MATLAB (Mathworks, Inc., Sherborn, MA). Analyses focused on changes in blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast that occurred during anticipation and outcome in the monetary reward condition of the MID task. Preprocessing included spatial realignment, unwarping, slice-time correction, spatial normalization into standard Montreal Neurological Institute space based on the segmented anatomical scan and spatially smoothening with an 8 mm full width half-maximum Gaussian kernel.

First and second level analyses

Changes in the BOLD signal for each subject (i.e. ‘activation’) were tested on the basis of linear combinations of estimated GLM parameters (beta values). These changes, displayed by individual contrast images, correspond to the percent signal change (effect size). First level analyses were performed by modeling two separate reward regressors for each trial type: anticipation period and outcome period (40). Realignment parameters were included in the statistical model as additional regressors to correct for motion effects. Then, contrast images were calculated to assess reward anticipation (reward trial anticipation > neutral trial anticipation) and reward outcome (reward trial hit > reward trial miss). Next, to compute group-level statistics, these individual contrast images were entered into second-level analyses. The second-level data were then extracted from MATLAB and used to perform multiple regression analyses in SPSS.

Statistical analyses

Power calculations were conducted using G-Power software (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) (Version 3.1.9.7). For the multiple linear regression analysis, we considered small (F2 = 0.02), medium (F2 = 0.15), and large (F2 = 0.35) effect sizes, accounting for 12 predictors and a corrected alpha level of 0.008. The estimated power was 0.03 for small effect sizes, 0.54 for medium effect sizes, and 0.98 for large effect sizes, respectively. Zero-order correlations were calculated to inspect the relationship between the variables. These indicated that the neural activity during the tasks in the VS, amygdala and mPFC was highly correlated across hemispheres (see correlation matrix, online Supplementary Table S1; for main effects of the MID task, see online Supplementary Figure S1). In our analyses, we therefore utilized the average activation of regions of interest (i.e. total VS) to examine the functional brain correlates. Then, to reduce structural multicollinearity between the main effects of the predictors and their interaction terms and to facilitate interpretation of our parameter estimates, we mean-centered all predictors. Second, for all predictors and for the DBD outcome measure, outliers were defined as the scores deviating more than three S.D. from the mean. These outliers (N = 6) were constrained at their lower and upper boundaries (mean + /- three S.D.), so that no extreme score carried excessive weight. We applied a square root transformation of the DBD variable, to obtain a non-significant departure from normality (see online Supplementary Figure SI). We observed only minor differences between the outcomes before and after transformation, therefore, for the purpose of interpretation, we report the standardized regression coefficients and p-values for the untransformed DBD score throughout this study. We then ran a multiple linear regression model to test for the interaction effect of DPA and reward sensitivity on DBD outcome, correcting for the possible confounders sex, SES, age, and the covariate interaction terms. Since reward sensitivity was indexed by the neural response in three brain regions (striatum, amygdala, and mPFC), during two conditions (reward anticipation and outcome), six separate multiple linear regression analyses – each including twelve predictors – were run. In order to account for multiple testing, the threshold for significance of the interaction terms was corrected for 6 independent tests and hence chosen as p < 0.00833. Lastly, in case of nominally significant ( p < 0.05) interaction of a covariate and DPA on DBD, we performed separate follow-up analyses in strata defined by the covariate to further explore the relationship between DPA and DBD.

Results

In this cross-sectional study, we observed a significant positive correlation between DPA and DBD (rs = 0.55, p < 0.001), but not between any of the ROI’s reward-related neural activity and DPA or DBD ( p > 0.05).

Linear regression analyses showed a significant interaction effect of DPA with VS responsiveness on DBD during reward outcome (β = 0.314, p < 0.001). In addition, our final model revealed a significant interaction effect of DPA with age (β = −0.267, p = 0.006). No interaction effect was found between DPA and VS responsiveness during reward anticipation ( p > 0.05, see online Supplementary Table S2). Moreover, additional testing did not reveal evidence for an interaction effect of DPA with amygdala and mPFC responsiveness on DBD during reward anticipation and outcome ( p > 0.05, see online Supplementary Table S3–S6) (Table 1).

Screenshot 2024-10-23 at 22.18.21

To further investigate the interactions, participants were categorized into three groups based on their neural activity in the ventral striatum (VS) during the reward outcome phase. The group division was determined using quartiles, resulting in a low VS group (N = 31), a middle VS group (N = 64), and a high VS group (N = 31). Subsequent post-hoc analyses revealed a strengthening association between DPA and DBD as VS responsiveness increased: lowest quartile VS group correlation (r) = 0.49, p = 0.005), middle quartile VS group (r = 0.56, p < 0.001) high quartile VS group (r = 0.64, p < 0.001).

Sex and age-specific effects on the DPA-DBD relationship

Sex-specific analyses revealed a significant positive association between DPA and DBD in males (r(105) = 0.59, p < 0.001), while no such association was observed in females (r(17) = 0.28, p = 0.25). Age-specific analyses indicated a diminishing association with increasing age: the lowest age group (12–16) exhibited a strong correlation (r(36) = 0.72, p < 0.001), followed by the middle age group (17–18) with a moderate correlation (r(59) = 0.57, p < 0.001), and the highest age group (19–20) showing a weaker correlation (r(25) = 0.30, p = 0.13).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this study presents the first evidence of an interaction effect between a brain mechanism and an environmental factor in relation to DBD symptom severity. We found that the link between affiliation with delinquent peers and DBD symptom scores was associated with reward sensitivity in the VS (i.e. striatal reward responsiveness), with the association being stronger in individuals displaying increased VS response to reward compared to those displaying a low response.

The present study found a significant positive correlation between DPA and DBD, which is in line with developmental theories proposing that affiliation with delinquent peers and susceptibility to peer influence are important contributors to adolescent delinquency. Although prior studies have reported that genetic variation, such as MAOA (Lu & Menard, 2017) and GABRA2 (Villafuerte, Trucco, Heitzeg, Burmeister, & Zucker, 2014), could moderate peer influences on externalizing or antisocial behavior, such candidate gene studies have not been replicated well (Dick et al., 2015; Duncan & Keller, 2011). Burt and Klump (2013) found that DPA was an etiological moderator of childhood delinquency and demonstrated that not shared genetic, but shared environmental influences on delinquency were larger in higher levels of DPA as compared to those with lower levels of DPA, indicating socialization effects are stronger than selection effects (Burt & Klump, 2013). The main effect of DPA and its interaction effect with reward sensitivity in this study were only present in males. It should be noted however, that the power to detect an interaction effect was limited in the female-specific sample (N = 19). A previous meta-analysis in 277 studies did not find evidence (d = 0.01) for sex-differences in reward sensitivity (Cross, Copping, & Campbell, 2011). Still, others have found higher scores in males on antisocial peer conformity vignettes, but no sex-differences on the neutral peer pressure subscales, indicating that the absence of an interaction effect in females could be due to differences in sensitivity specifically towards antisocial peer affiliation mechanisms (Santor, Messervey, & Kusumakar, n.d.).

The present study did not find a significant association between DBD symptom scores and reward-related neural activity. In an earlier study, carried out in the same sample, Cohn et al. (2015) found that persistent DBD subjects were associated with lower neural responses in the VS compared to desisting DBD and healthy subjects. This finding suggests that persistent DBD subjects may be less sensitive to the socializing effects of its peers and selection mechanisms might have a stronger effect on the association between DPA and DBD in this distinct group.

We only found evidence of an interaction effect for responsiveness of the VS to reward and not for reward anticipation. Previous studies have reported associations between VS response and antisocial behaviors, including increased activation of the VS already during the anticipation phase (Huettel, Stowe, Gordon, Warner, & Platt, 2006; Knutson et al., 2001a). Van Leijenhorst et al. (2010) have argued that in those experiments, since the participants can to some extent predict that a reward will follow, the neural activity is merely an early excitement response rather than anticipating a possible reward (Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010). In their study, using a task design that did not allow for reward prediction, the peak in VS activation was only observed during the actual delivery of rewards. Glenn and Yang (2012) proposed that the increased VS response observed in antisocial and psychopathic individuals may not necessarily indicate hypersensitivity in reward processing, but a failure to signal the absence of a reward, which in turn may lead to enduring maladaptive behaviors (Glenn & Yang, 2012).

Our findings indicate that individual differences in neural reward processing may be linked to the sensitivity towards peer affiliation, when examining their effects on DBD. Therapeutic interventions focusing on hypersensitive adolescents may thus specifically target peer affiliation to modify the environment by withholding adolescents from negative peer influences and promoting positive, healthy peer interactions.

Limitations and future directions

Our results underscore the multifaceted nature of brain-behavior relationships and the importance of considering neural specificity, individual variability, and sample characteristics when interpreting research outcomes. We did not find evidence for an interaction effect between DPA and amygdala or mPFC responsiveness during reward anticipation and outcome. It is plausible that the association between DPA and disruptive behavior may primarily involve the VS, as this region is more closely tied to social reward sensitivity and reinforcement learning relevant to affiliating with delinquent peers. In contrast, the amygdala and mPFC may not be as directly involved in the specific social reward aspects related to DPA in this context, which could explain the absence of an interaction effect with these regions. At the same time, brain-behavior relationships often involve multiple factors, and it is possible that other unmeasured variables or individual differences contributed to the lack of an observed interaction with the amygdala and mPFC. Further research with larger samples and targeted paradigms could enhance statistical power and sensitivity to detect potential interactions with the amygdala and mPFC. Moreover, in future studies, comparing brain responses of delinquent adolescents with a control group will reveal if VS interactions are specific to this population or generalize to others without delinquent affiliations. Even though our study finds strong support for a biosocial mechanism, the proportion of explained variance is limited, which is expected given the variety of other factors that play a role. In addition, research in twins has demonstrated shared genetic liability of DPA and conduct problems, indicating a gene-environment correlation (Button et al., 2007). Strikingly, the genetic correlation between DPA and conduct problems was found to be context-dependent in this study, such that the correlation increased with higher DPA scores. This GxE correlation offers the possibility of selection mechanisms explaining the correlation between DPA and DBD and can potentially confound the estimated interaction effect in our study, by increasing the type I error rate. Future studies examining the interplay between DPA and DBD should thus consider both plausible mechanisms to inform prevention strategies. We acknowledge the potential influence of substance use on our findings, given the literature linking reward sensitivity to substance use during adolescence (Obando, Trujillo, & Trujillo, 2014) and its common co-occurrence with antisocial peers (Andrews, Tildesley, Hops, & Li, 2002) and DBD (Bukstein, 2000). While we were unable to directly control for substance use in our analyses due to data limitations, we recognize its potential significance in shaping the observed relationships. Substance use may act as a mediating or moderating factor, influencing the neurobiological processes underlying disruptive behavior (Andrews & Hops, 2010). Given this complexity, future research should incorporate substance use data into investigations exploring the interplay between antisocial peers, reward sensitivity, and DBD symptoms.

Moreover, to reliably capture the concept of reward-sensitivity, it should ideally be examined in different modalities, both behaviorally (through questionnaires and cognitive tasks) and biologically (genetic risk score). Our study was not designed to formally test the differentially susceptibility theory, which proposes that neurobiological characteristics may determine who will benefit disproportionally from positive environmental factors, while being also more vulnerable to negative environments (Belsky & Pluess, 2009). We thus encourage future research to examine whether individuals differ in their susceptibility to both the antisocial and prosocial effects of peer affiliation on DBD symptom severity. Moreover, longitudinal studies could explore striatal reactivity to reward and sensitivity to DPA during preadolescence and then follow-up with DBD at middle and late adolescence. Future studies could also examine differences in the neural underpinnings of social and monetary incentive processing (Greimel et al., 2018). Ideally, since reward-sensitivity is a rather broad and indirect measure of susceptibility, future studies could focus on peer susceptibility instead, to achieve a more ecologically valid interaction design. Additionally, despite successfully obtaining unique fMRI data from a relatively large at-risk sample, power analyses demonstrated that detecting small or subtle effects remains a challenge, also due to the number of predictors, including interaction variables. Lastly, we recognize the broader challenges in fMRI studies as highlighted by recent literature (Elliott et al., 2020). Task fMRI measures have demonstrated poor reliability and limited test-retest reliabilities in a priori regions of interest. We therefore remain cautious in our interpretations and recognize the need for replication and validation in independent samples to strengthen the generalizability of our findings.

To conclude, our results demonstrate the potential of brain-environment interaction studies (with careful methodological designs), as a powerful tool to advance our understanding of adolescent risk-taking and deviant behaviors. Intervention studies aimed at enriching peer-to-peer interactions may serve as a buffering strategy against disruptive behaviors, particularly for reward-sensitive adolescents.

Abstract

Affiliating with delinquent peers may stimulate the development of antisocial behavior, especially for adolescents who are sensitive to social rewards. The current study examines whether the association between delinquent peer affiliation (DPA) and disruptive behavior interacts with functional brain correlates of reward sensitivity in early onset male adolescents delinquents. Childhood arrestees (n = 126, mean age = 17.7 [S.D. 1.6]) completed a DPA questionnaire, and participated in an fMRI study in which reward sensitivity was operationalized through responsiveness of the ventral striatum (VS), amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during the monetary incentive delay paradigm (reward anticipation and outcome). Symptoms of disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) were assessed through structured psychiatric interviews (Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children) with adolescents. DPA had a main effect on DBD symptoms. Adolescents with high VS reward responses showed a stronger significant positive association between DPA and DBD symptoms compared to low VS responders. No evidence for an interaction effect was found for the amygdala and mPFC. Post-hoc analyses revealed the positive association between DPA and DBD was only present in males, with a diminishing effect as age increased. We found evidence for a biosocial interaction between DPA and reward sensitivity of the VS in relation to DBD symptom severity. This study provides the first evidence of an interaction effect between a brain mechanism and an environmental factor in relation to DBD symptoms, implying that susceptibility to influences of delinquent peers may intertwine with individual biological differences.

Summary

This study investigates the interaction between brain mechanisms and environmental factors in relation to disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) in adolescents. Delinquent peer affiliation (DPA) is a significant environmental risk factor for DBD, but the nature of this relationship is complex. The study examines the role of reward sensitivity in the ventral striatum (VS), amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in mediating the relationship between DPA and DBD.

Delinquent peer affiliation and DBD

DPA is a well-established risk factor for DBD. However, the association is likely due to a combination of causal socialization effects and non-causal selection processes. Individual susceptibility to peer influence is moderated by factors such as callous and unemotional traits, prosocial involvement, and resistance to peer influence.

Adolescence and reward processing

Adolescence is marked by significant neurobiological changes, including a hyper-responsive reward system. These changes may predispose adolescents to increased sensitivity to reward-related cues, such as cooperative peer-interaction.

Susceptibility to peer influence, brain function, and risk taking behavior

Neuroimaging studies have shown that the mesolimbic dopamine reward system is activated in response to peer interactions, particularly cooperative and socially shared contexts. Individual differences in dopaminergic neurotransmission may lead to different responses to environmental cues, such as peer influences. Antisocial adolescents often exhibit altered reward sensitivity, with heightened dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens during reinforcement.

Materials and methods

The study utilized data from a sample of first-time arrestees in the Netherlands, aged 17.7 years on average. Delinquent peer affiliation (DPA), DBD symptom scores, and reward sensitivity were assessed through questionnaires and a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task.

Results

The study found a significant positive correlation between DPA and DBD. A significant interaction effect of DPA with VS responsiveness on DBD during reward outcome was observed, suggesting that the link between DPA and DBD is stronger in individuals with increased VS response to reward. The interaction effect was particularly pronounced in males and in younger adolescents.

Discussion

The study provides evidence for an interaction effect between a brain mechanism and an environmental factor in relation to DBD severity. The findings suggest that individual differences in neural reward processing may influence sensitivity towards peer affiliation. This has implications for therapeutic interventions, which could target peer affiliation and promote positive peer interactions, particularly for adolescents with increased reward sensitivity.

Limitations and future directions

The study is limited by the cross-sectional design and the relatively small sample size, particularly for females. Future research is needed to explore the interaction effects of DPA and reward sensitivity in a longitudinal study with a larger sample. Additionally, the study focused on reward sensitivity, but further investigation of peer susceptibility is warranted. Moreover, future studies should consider substance use and examine the interplay between DPA, reward sensitivity, and DBD symptoms. Further research with diverse samples and targeted paradigms is essential to strengthen the generalizability of the findings.

Abstract

Affiliating with delinquent peers may stimulate the development of antisocial behavior, especially for adolescents who are sensitive to social rewards. The current study examines whether the association between delinquent peer affiliation (DPA) and disruptive behavior interacts with functional brain correlates of reward sensitivity in early onset male adolescents delinquents. Childhood arrestees (n = 126, mean age = 17.7 [S.D. 1.6]) completed a DPA questionnaire, and participated in an fMRI study in which reward sensitivity was operationalized through responsiveness of the ventral striatum (VS), amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during the monetary incentive delay paradigm (reward anticipation and outcome). Symptoms of disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) were assessed through structured psychiatric interviews (Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children) with adolescents. DPA had a main effect on DBD symptoms. Adolescents with high VS reward responses showed a stronger significant positive association between DPA and DBD symptoms compared to low VS responders. No evidence for an interaction effect was found for the amygdala and mPFC. Post-hoc analyses revealed the positive association between DPA and DBD was only present in males, with a diminishing effect as age increased. We found evidence for a biosocial interaction between DPA and reward sensitivity of the VS in relation to DBD symptom severity. This study provides the first evidence of an interaction effect between a brain mechanism and an environmental factor in relation to DBD symptoms, implying that susceptibility to influences of delinquent peers may intertwine with individual biological differences.

Summary

This study explores the relationship between delinquent peer affiliation (DPA), disruptive behavior disorder (DBD) symptoms, and reward sensitivity in the brain. Researchers investigated whether individual differences in brain responses to rewards could explain why some adolescents are more susceptible to the influence of delinquent peers.

Delinquent peer affiliation and DBD

DPA is a significant risk factor for DBD, but the nature of their relationship is complex. Two theories exist: (1) causal socializing effects, where affiliating with deviant peers leads to increased disruptive behavior, and (2) non-causal selection processes, where disruptive individuals choose to affiliate with disruptive peers. While both processes likely play a role, their influence varies with age. Individual factors like callous-unemotional traits and prosocial involvement also moderate the relationship.

Adolescence and reward processing

Adolescence is characterized by heightened social interactions, risk-taking, impulsivity, and reward sensitivity, driven by neurobiological and hormonal changes. The dopamine system undergoes significant structural changes during this period, leading to a hyper-responsive reward system in the brain. Neuroimaging studies show that adolescents exhibit greater recruitment of limbic systems (involved in reward processing) compared to cortical control systems. This imbalance can make them more sensitive to reward-related cues.

Susceptibility to peer influence, brain function, and risk taking behavior

Neuroimaging studies have linked activation of the mesolimbic dopamine reward system to peer interactions, particularly cooperative and shared contexts. Individual differences in dopaminergic neurotransmission can lead to varied responses to environmental cues, including peer influence. Notably, adolescents with delinquent peers exhibit amplified activity in reward-related brain regions, promoting risky choices.

Previous research has also identified differences in reward sensitivity between antisocial adolescents and healthy controls, suggesting potential impairments in the cortico-striatal circuit. The amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) are crucial for reward processing and have been implicated in mediating the effects of stress on reward sensitivity.

Materials and methods

This cross-sectional study utilized data from a cohort of childhood-onset offenders. Participants were assessed for DBD symptoms, DPA, and underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during a monetary incentive delay (MID) task to measure reward sensitivity. The MID task involves anticipating potential monetary rewards and responding to cues. fMRI data were analyzed to identify brain regions involved in reward processing.

Results

The study revealed a significant positive correlation between DPA and DBD, indicating that adolescents who affiliate with delinquent peers are more likely to exhibit DBD symptoms. However, no direct association was found between reward-related neural activity and either DPA or DBD.

Crucially, a significant interaction effect was found between DPA and reward sensitivity in the ventral striatum (VS) during reward outcome. This suggests that the relationship between DPA and DBD is stronger in individuals with heightened VS response to reward. Notably, this interaction was only observed in males. Further analysis showed that as VS responsiveness increased, the correlation between DPA and DBD strengthened.

Sex and age-specific effects on the DPA-DBD relationship

Sex-specific analyses revealed a strong association between DPA and DBD in males but not in females. Age-specific analyses showed a diminishing association with increasing age.

Discussion

This study provides evidence for an interaction between a brain mechanism (reward sensitivity in the VS) and an environmental factor (DPA) in relation to DBD symptoms. This finding suggests that individual differences in reward sensitivity may influence vulnerability to the influence of delinquent peers.

The study's limitations include the cross-sectional design, potential confounding factors, and the need for further investigation into the role of the amygdala and mPFC. Despite these limitations, the findings provide valuable insights into the neurobiological underpinnings of DBD and the potential for targeted interventions.

Limitations and future directions

Future research should address the study's limitations, including incorporating longitudinal studies, examining the role of substance use, and utilizing a broader range of reward-related measures. Additionally, researchers should investigate whether individuals differ in their susceptibility to both the antisocial and prosocial effects of peer affiliation.

The findings highlight the potential of brain-environment interaction studies to advance our understanding of adolescent risk-taking and deviant behaviors. Interventions that focus on enriching peer-to-peer interactions may be beneficial for reward-sensitive adolescents, potentially mitigating the negative effects of delinquent peer affiliation.

Abstract

Affiliating with delinquent peers may stimulate the development of antisocial behavior, especially for adolescents who are sensitive to social rewards. The current study examines whether the association between delinquent peer affiliation (DPA) and disruptive behavior interacts with functional brain correlates of reward sensitivity in early onset male adolescents delinquents. Childhood arrestees (n = 126, mean age = 17.7 [S.D. 1.6]) completed a DPA questionnaire, and participated in an fMRI study in which reward sensitivity was operationalized through responsiveness of the ventral striatum (VS), amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during the monetary incentive delay paradigm (reward anticipation and outcome). Symptoms of disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) were assessed through structured psychiatric interviews (Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children) with adolescents. DPA had a main effect on DBD symptoms. Adolescents with high VS reward responses showed a stronger significant positive association between DPA and DBD symptoms compared to low VS responders. No evidence for an interaction effect was found for the amygdala and mPFC. Post-hoc analyses revealed the positive association between DPA and DBD was only present in males, with a diminishing effect as age increased. We found evidence for a biosocial interaction between DPA and reward sensitivity of the VS in relation to DBD symptom severity. This study provides the first evidence of an interaction effect between a brain mechanism and an environmental factor in relation to DBD symptoms, implying that susceptibility to influences of delinquent peers may intertwine with individual biological differences.

Summary

This study examines how the brain responds to reward and its connection to disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) in adolescents. The researchers investigated whether the strength of the link between DBD and delinquent peer affiliation (DPA) was related to how sensitive adolescents were to reward. They found that adolescents who were more sensitive to reward were more likely to develop DBD symptoms, especially when they had close friends who engaged in delinquent behavior.

Delinquent peer affiliation and DBD

Spending time with delinquent peers is a significant risk factor for developing DBD. Studies show that adolescents with delinquent friends are more likely to commit crimes and develop antisocial personality disorder later in life. Researchers have suggested that this link is due to both socialization (learning delinquent behavior from friends) and selection (individuals with DBD choosing to be friends with other disruptive individuals). However, not all adolescents are equally influenced by their peers. For example, adolescents with traits like callousness or low prosocial involvement are more susceptible to peer influence.

Adolescence and reward processing

Adolescence is a time of heightened social interaction, risk-taking, and impulsivity. This is partly because the brain undergoes significant changes during this period, including increased sensitivity to reward.

Susceptibility to peer influence, brain function, and risk taking behavior

Brain regions involved in reward processing (the ventral striatum, amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex) play a role in how adolescents respond to social cues. Studies show that adolescents who have more active reward centers in their brain, particularly the ventral striatum, are more likely to engage in risky behavior when around their peers.

Materials and methods

The study used data from a group of adolescents who had been arrested for crimes. The researchers assessed their DBD symptoms, their level of affiliation with delinquent peers, and their brain activity during a task that measured their sensitivity to reward.

Results

The study found that there was a strong positive correlation between delinquent peer affiliation and DBD symptoms. They also found that the relationship between DBD and delinquent peer affiliation was stronger in adolescents who showed a greater response in their ventral striatum to reward. This suggests that adolescents with more sensitive reward centers in their brain may be more vulnerable to the influence of their peers, especially if those peers are engaging in delinquent behavior.

Discussion

The findings suggest that individual differences in reward sensitivity might explain why some adolescents are more susceptible to the influence of their peers. This knowledge could be useful for developing interventions that focus on reducing the influence of delinquent peers, particularly for adolescents who are more sensitive to reward.

Limitations and future directions

This study had several limitations:

  • The study did not find an interaction effect between delinquent peer affiliation and amygdala or medial prefrontal cortex responsiveness. This could be due to the specific brain regions involved in reward processing, or other factors.

  • The study relied on a limited sample of adolescents who had been arrested for crimes. This means that the results may not generalize to all adolescents with DBD.

  • Substance use, which can impact both reward sensitivity and delinquent behavior, was not fully accounted for in the study.

Future studies could address these limitations by using larger samples, investigating other brain regions, and including substance use data. Additionally, future research could examine the interplay between genetic and environmental factors that influence the development of DBD, and explore how individual differences in susceptibility to both positive and negative peer influence might impact DBD symptom severity.

Conclusion

This study provides evidence for a biosocial mechanism that may contribute to the development of DBD symptoms in adolescents. The findings suggest that individual differences in brain responses to reward may play a role in susceptibility to peer influence, which in turn may affect the development of DBD. This research highlights the importance of addressing both brain function and environmental factors in understanding and treating DBD.

Abstract

Affiliating with delinquent peers may stimulate the development of antisocial behavior, especially for adolescents who are sensitive to social rewards. The current study examines whether the association between delinquent peer affiliation (DPA) and disruptive behavior interacts with functional brain correlates of reward sensitivity in early onset male adolescents delinquents. Childhood arrestees (n = 126, mean age = 17.7 [S.D. 1.6]) completed a DPA questionnaire, and participated in an fMRI study in which reward sensitivity was operationalized through responsiveness of the ventral striatum (VS), amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) during the monetary incentive delay paradigm (reward anticipation and outcome). Symptoms of disruptive behavior disorders (DBD) were assessed through structured psychiatric interviews (Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children) with adolescents. DPA had a main effect on DBD symptoms. Adolescents with high VS reward responses showed a stronger significant positive association between DPA and DBD symptoms compared to low VS responders. No evidence for an interaction effect was found for the amygdala and mPFC. Post-hoc analyses revealed the positive association between DPA and DBD was only present in males, with a diminishing effect as age increased. We found evidence for a biosocial interaction between DPA and reward sensitivity of the VS in relation to DBD symptom severity. This study provides the first evidence of an interaction effect between a brain mechanism and an environmental factor in relation to DBD symptoms, implying that susceptibility to influences of delinquent peers may intertwine with individual biological differences.

Summary

This research looked at how a teen's brain and their friends can affect how likely they are to get in trouble. The study found that teens who are more sensitive to rewards in their brain are more likely to get in trouble if their friends are also getting in trouble.

Delinquent peer affiliation and DBD

Teens who hang out with friends who get in trouble are more likely to get in trouble themselves. Scientists think this happens because those friends influence the teen to do bad things. However, some teens are more easily influenced by their friends than others.

Adolescence and reward processing

Teens' brains are still developing, and they are especially sensitive to rewards. This means that they are more likely to do things that make them feel good, even if those things are risky.

Susceptibility to peer influence, brain function, and risk taking behavior

Studies show that when teens think about rewards, parts of their brain that are involved in feeling good are more active. Teens who have this strong response to rewards in their brains may be more likely to get in trouble if they hang out with friends who are also getting in trouble.

Materials and methods

This study used brain scans and questionnaires to see how teens' brains respond to rewards and how often they get in trouble. The study also looked at how often teens hang out with friends who get in trouble.

Subjects

The study included teens who had gotten in trouble with the law. The teens were all about 18 years old.

Measurements and procedure

The teens were asked questions about how often they get in trouble and how often they hang out with friends who get in trouble. They also had their brains scanned while they played a game that involved rewards.

fMRI acquisition

The teens’ brains were scanned while they played a game. This allowed researchers to see how their brains respond to rewards.

fMRI analysis

The brain scans were analyzed to see which parts of the brain were most active during the game.

First and second level analyses

The brain scans were analyzed to see how active different parts of the brain were when the teens were expecting a reward and when they got a reward.

Statistical analyses

The data from the brain scans and questionnaires was analyzed to see if there was a connection between how active the teens’ brains were during the reward game, how often they hung out with friends who get in trouble, and how often they got in trouble themselves.

Results

The study found that teens who are more sensitive to rewards in their brains are more likely to get in trouble if their friends are also getting in trouble. This was especially true for boys. The study also found that the connection between hanging out with friends who get in trouble and getting in trouble themselves was weaker for older teens.

Discussion

This study shows that teens who are more sensitive to rewards in their brains may be more easily influenced by their friends who are also getting in trouble. This information can help scientists understand how teen behavior develops and how to help teens stay out of trouble.

Limitations and future directions

This study was limited because it only looked at teens who had gotten in trouble with the law. Future research should include a group of teens who have not gotten in trouble with the law to see if the findings are the same. It would also be helpful to look at other factors that might influence teen behavior, such as family life and school experiences.

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Tielbeek, J. J., van der Hooft, M., Cohn, M. D., Van De Ven, P. M., Polderman, T. J., Veltman, D. J., ... & Popma, A. (2024). The association between delinquent peer affiliation and disruptive behavior interacts with functional brain correlates of reward sensitivity: A biosocial interaction study in adolescent delinquents. Psychological medicine, 54(8), 1544-1551. https://doi:10.1017/S0033291723003380

    Highlights