Responsible Use of Open-Access Developmental Data: the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (abcd) Study
Cortney Simmons
May I. Conley
Dylan G. Gee
Arielle Baskin-Sommers
Deanna M. Barch
SimpleOriginal

Summary

Open data sharing in research enhances collaboration and accuracy. Responsible use involves understanding data sources, particularly for studies involving children's brain development and environmental contexts.

2021

Responsible Use of Open-Access Developmental Data: the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (abcd) Study

Keywords Children; development; brain; behavior; neuroscience; psychology; review

Abstract

The number and size of open-access developmental data sets that include brain and behavioral information have dramatically increased in recent years (Fig. 1; see also Table S1 in the Supplemental Material). These collaborative initiatives represent a new era of science that democratizes data access, facilitates scientific discovery, boosts statistical power, enhances reproducibility and replication, and can inform policy (Rosenberg et al., 2018). However, to use these data responsibly, we must consider the broader social context, the dynamic and interactive process of development, and the strength and limitations of the data when formulating our research questions, designing statistical models, and interpreting our findings. Here, we use the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, the largest longitudinal study of brain development and youth health in the United States to date, to suggest best practices for responsible data use. To be developmentally informed and responsible users of these data, we must consider (a) the heterogeneity of experiences within the broader social context in which development occurs and (b) the potential for adaptation to the environment and developmental change.

Introduction

The number and size of open-access developmental data sets that include brain and behavioral information have dramatically increased in recent years (Fig. 1; see also Table S1 in the Supplemental Material). These collaborative initiatives represent a new era of science that democratizes data access, facilitates scientific discovery, boosts statistical power, enhances reproducibility and replication, and can inform policy (Rosenberg et al., 2018). However, to use these data responsibly, we must consider the broader social context, the dynamic and interactive process of development, and the strength and limitations of the data when formulating our research questions, designing statistical models, and interpreting our findings. Here, we use the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, the largest longitudinal study of brain development and youth health in the United States to date, to suggest best practices for responsible data use.

Screenshot 2024-06-02 at 9.13.28 PM

Fig. 1.

Existing, ongoing, or planned data sets including structural and functional neuroimaging data from approximately 500 or more children or adolescents. These data sets, which represent both prospective and retrospective samples, are the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study (ABCD; United States); Healthy Brain Network (HBN; United States); Lifespan Human Connectome Project Development (HCP-D; United States); National Consortium on Alcohol and Neurodevelopment in Adolescence (NCANDA; United States); Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics (PING; United States); Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort (PNC; United States); Saguenay Youth Study (SYS; Canada); High Risk Cohort Study for the Development of Childhood Psychiatric Disorders (HRC; Brazil); Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange (ABIDE; United States, Germany, Ireland, Belgium, The Netherlands); Enhancing Neuro Imaging Genetics Through Meta Analysis (ENIGMA; worldwide); IMAGEN (England, Ireland, France, Germany); Youth of Utrecht (YOUth; part of the Consortium on Individual Development, or CID; The Netherlands); Generation R (Gen R) Study (The Netherlands); NeuroIMAGE (follow-up of the Dutch arm of the International Multi-Centre ADHD Genetics, or IMAGE, project; The Netherlands); Consortium on Vulnerability to Externalizing Disorders and Addictions (c-VEDA; United Kingdom, India); Consortium for Reliability and Reproducibility (CoRR; China, United States, Canada, Germany); ADHD-200 (United States, China); and HEALthy Brain and Child Development Study (HBCD; United States). Although samples are distributed across the globe, African, Middle Eastern, South Asian, Oceanian, and Central and South American populations are underrepresented. Data-collection efforts in these regions and others will be important for ensuring diverse, representative samples that will allow researchers to uncover general principles of the developing brain. (The map outline is courtesy of Wikimedia user Loadfile and is edited with permission from Rosenberg et al., 2018.)

To be developmentally informed and responsible users of these data, we must consider (a) the heterogeneity of experiences within the broader social context in which development occurs and (b) the potential for adaptation to the environment and developmental change.

Consider the Broader Social Context in Which Development Occurs

The ABCD Study focuses on a cohort of nearly 12,000 participants who were 9 to 10 years old at baseline and are currently being followed for 10 years. Youths and their families provide rich details about their environmental experiences and undergo extensive phenotypic, cognitive, genetic, emotional, health, and neuroimaging assessments. These data provide an unprecedented opportunity to advance our understanding of how the culmination of different experiences interact with changing biology across development. Yet the ABCD Study does not capture the full spectrum of experiences or environments that may influence a youth’s development (e.g., structural inequality). Moreover, the sample is not representative of the U.S. population on sex, race, ethnicity, education, and household income (Compton et al., 2019; Dick et al., 2021; Garavan et al., 2018), which can limit the ability to generalize findings across and between youths from diverse groups (cf. Heeringa & Berglund, 2020). Therefore, it is important to consider measured and unmeasured meso-level factors (e.g., neighborhood influences) and macro-level factors (e.g., systemic or structural factors) when examining developmental trajectories or outcomes, particularly with open-access developmental data sets that may be used by scientists from many disciplines.

Development does not occur in a vacuum and is influenced by complex multifaceted processes. Youths live in multiple social contexts (e.g., family, neighborhood, region) and hold multiple identities (e.g., racial, cultural, gender). Therefore, studies that focus on single environmental factors (e.g., median neighborhood income) fail to capture the considerable heterogeneity in youths’ experiences and environments (Cohodes et al., 2021; Finkelhor et al., 2007) and their contributions to development (Foulkes & Blakemore, 2018; Hong et al., 2021). Large, publicly accessible data sets such as the ABCD Study include a wealth of assessments capturing different aspects of youths’ experiences (e.g., culture and environments) that may relate to developmental outcomes (Barch et al., 2018; Hoffman et al., 2019; Zucker et al., 2018). In addition to acknowledging that no measures will fully capture the relevant environment or experiences, we should consider utilizing multifactorial or multiple separate variables to estimate context and experience to enhance accuracy in measurement and interpretation. Further, we should avoid using any single variable (e.g., race, which is a social construct; Trent et al., 2019) as a proxy for other sociodemographic issues (e.g., marginalization) and avoid overly simplistic conclusions when examining development.

Structural and cultural racism, sexism, and other forms of oppression and inequity shape youths’ day-to-day experiences and influence their development (Neblett, 2019; Syed et al., 2018). Although the ABCD data include some self- and parent-report assessments of youths’ exposure to different forms of inequality (e.g., discrimination, resource insecurity, neighborhood disadvantage), many of the manifestations of structural inequality, privilege, and power are not measured (Cole, 2009; del Río-González et al., 2021). It is therefore important to situate our research in the broader social context and exercise caution when making claims about developmental findings. We should be careful to acknowledge unmeasured factors that may influence our findings given that not all critical factors are measured.

Consider the Potential for Adaptation to the Environment and Developmental Change

Development is a dynamic, interactive process in which a youth’s changing biology adapts to environmental challenges encountered in various developmental stages and contexts (Gottlieb, 1991; Greenough et al., 1987; Karmiloff-Smith, 2009). When there is a deviation in the expected environment (e.g., absence of primary caregivers, childhood maltreatment, institutionalized care), some evolutionarily conserved mechanisms may be maladaptive for that current environment (Casey et al., 2010). Thus, outcomes that could be interpreted as unfavorable in one environment may actually reflect an adaptive process suited to meet the demands of another environment (Amso, 2020). It is therefore important to acknowledge that a given outcome may be favorable or unfavorable depending on the individual’s current and/or past environment.

Moreover, how youths respond to the same environment or experience can be quite different. For example, although some early life experiences (e.g., physical abuse, exposure to community violence) can increase risk for psychopathology, many children who experience adversity do not develop mental health or behavioral problems (Kessler et al., 2010; Masten, 2001). The heterogeneity in outcomes following certain early life experiences highlights the limits in using variables within only one level of analysis (e.g., environment, genetics) as predictive criteria. Recent advances in predictive modeling underscore the need to utilize variables across multiple levels simultaneously so as to capture the youth more holistically (Rosenberg et al., 2018). However, even when multilevel approaches are used, accuracy of prediction is low across developmental periods (e.g., using childhood experiences to predict adult behavior; Salganik et al., 2020). Therefore, we should be cautious when using predictive modeling. Furthermore, given the limitations of predictions across developmental stages, we must recognize the potential real-life implications of making such predictions (e.g., stigmatization that results in the narrowing of opportunities).

Youths are constantly adapting, and change remains possible. Research shows that the brain is plastic throughout life, with heightened potential for change during sensitive periods (Bavelier et al., 2010; Fu & Zuo, 2011). Ample time in a different environment can have profound effects on outcomes (Chetty et al., 2016). And interventions can mitigate the effects of early-life experiences (Gee & Casey, 2015). Thus, deterministic claims should not be made on the basis of a single time point using cross-sectional observational data or on the basis of multiple time points within a circumscribed developmental period. Moreover, we should be modest with our conclusions about development and avoid deterministic language in our interpretation and communication of findings (e.g., “incorrigible youth,” “diminished” ability for change).

Best Practices for Responsible Use of Open-Access Developmental Data

Given our own knowledge of the data and of the rich array of factors that influence development, some of which may be unmeasured, we must approach our science with humility. We suggest the following best practices for responsible use of open-access developmental data:

  • Respect the youths and families whose lives form the basis of the research data.

  • Consider whether we and/or members of our research teams have the knowledge needed to thoughtfully and thoroughly address questions regarding the influence of social context on developmental outcomes.

  • Practice awareness and appreciation for the heterogeneity of youths’ experiences and environments when formulating developmental research questions and analyses.

  • Utilize multifactorial or multiple separate variables to estimate context and experience to enhance accuracy in measurement and interpretation.

  • Situate findings in the broader social context by noting that some important factors may not always be measured.

  • Avoid using causal or deterministic language, either explicit or implicit, based on a single time point using cross-sectional observational data or based on multiple time points within a circumscribed developmental period.

  • Be modest with conclusions regarding observed associations even when using large developmental data sets, given all youths’ potential for change.

  • Recognize that outcomes that may be viewed as unfavorable in one environment may actually reflect an adaptation in another environment.

  • Consider how our findings may be misinterpreted as deterministic, to the detriment of other groups or individuals.

Conclusion

With big data comes big responsibility. Large, open-access, longitudinal data sets with deep phenotyping (e.g., ABCD Study) provide the opportunity to conduct multifaceted research. However, we must consider the strengths and limitations of these data, be mindful of how we include data in statistical analyses, and be cautious about how we interpret the results of these statistical analyses even when they are conducted rigorously. This Data Brief underscores the importance of considering the heterogeneity of youths’ experiences within the broader social context in which development occurs. Development is a multifaceted and dynamic process, and researchers must also consider the potential for adaptation and change among all youths. These considerations may enhance accuracy in the use and interpretation of open-access developmental data and mitigate potentially harmful narratives. We as scientists have an opportunity and an obligation to use these data to inform policy and to promote positive change in academic, health, and social outcomes for all youths.

Link to Article

Abstract

The number and size of open-access developmental data sets that include brain and behavioral information have dramatically increased in recent years (Fig. 1; see also Table S1 in the Supplemental Material). These collaborative initiatives represent a new era of science that democratizes data access, facilitates scientific discovery, boosts statistical power, enhances reproducibility and replication, and can inform policy (Rosenberg et al., 2018). However, to use these data responsibly, we must consider the broader social context, the dynamic and interactive process of development, and the strength and limitations of the data when formulating our research questions, designing statistical models, and interpreting our findings. Here, we use the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, the largest longitudinal study of brain development and youth health in the United States to date, to suggest best practices for responsible data use. To be developmentally informed and responsible users of these data, we must consider (a) the heterogeneity of experiences within the broader social context in which development occurs and (b) the potential for adaptation to the environment and developmental change.

Responsible Use of Open-Access Developmental Data: Considerations for the Broader Social Context and Developmental Change

Consider the Broader Social Context in Which Development Occurs

The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study provides rich data on the experiences and development of nearly 12,000 youths. However, it is crucial to acknowledge that this study does not capture the full spectrum of experiences and environments that influence development.

Development occurs within multiple social contexts, and youths hold various identities that shape their experiences. Therefore, studies that focus solely on single environmental factors fail to account for the significant heterogeneity in youths' lives. It is essential to consider multiple factors or variables to enhance accuracy in measuring and interpreting the influence of context and experience on development.

Moreover, structural and cultural inequalities can profoundly impact youths' experiences and development. While the ABCD data includes some measures of inequality, it is important to recognize that many manifestations of these inequalities may not be captured. Therefore, researchers should situate their findings within the broader social context and acknowledge unmeasured factors that may influence outcomes.

Consider the Potential for Adaptation to the Environment and Developmental Change

Development is a dynamic process in which individuals adapt to environmental challenges. Outcomes that may be considered unfavorable in one environment may reflect adaptive responses in another. It is crucial to recognize that youths respond differently to the same environment or experience.

Furthermore, the brain remains plastic throughout life, allowing for significant change. Early-life experiences can have lasting effects, but interventions and environmental changes can mitigate these effects. Therefore, deterministic claims based on a single time point or a limited developmental period should be avoided.

Best Practices for Responsible Use of Open-Access Developmental Data

To ensure responsible use of open-access developmental data, researchers should:

  • Respect the youths and families who contribute to the data.

  • Consider the heterogeneity of youths' experiences and environments.

  • Use multiple variables to estimate context and experience.

  • Situate findings within the broader social context.

  • Avoid causal or deterministic language.

  • Recognize the potential for change and adaptation.

  • Be aware of potential misinterpretations of findings.

Conclusion

Open-access developmental data provides valuable opportunities for research. However, it is essential to consider the limitations of these data and to interpret findings cautiously. By considering the broader social context and the potential for adaptation and change, researchers can enhance the accuracy and impact of their work and promote positive outcomes for all youths.

Link to Article

Abstract

The number and size of open-access developmental data sets that include brain and behavioral information have dramatically increased in recent years (Fig. 1; see also Table S1 in the Supplemental Material). These collaborative initiatives represent a new era of science that democratizes data access, facilitates scientific discovery, boosts statistical power, enhances reproducibility and replication, and can inform policy (Rosenberg et al., 2018). However, to use these data responsibly, we must consider the broader social context, the dynamic and interactive process of development, and the strength and limitations of the data when formulating our research questions, designing statistical models, and interpreting our findings. Here, we use the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, the largest longitudinal study of brain development and youth health in the United States to date, to suggest best practices for responsible data use. To be developmentally informed and responsible users of these data, we must consider (a) the heterogeneity of experiences within the broader social context in which development occurs and (b) the potential for adaptation to the environment and developmental change.

Responsible Use of Developmental Data: Things to Keep in Mind

Consider the Different Life Experiences of Young People

The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study is a huge project that's following thousands of kids for 10 years. It's giving researchers a lot of information about how young people develop. But it's important to remember that not all kids have the same experiences.

Some kids grow up in wealthy families with lots of support, while others grow up in poverty or with other challenges. These different experiences can have a big impact on how kids develop. So, when researchers are looking at data from the ABCD Study, they need to keep in mind that the kids in the study come from all walks of life.

Understand That Kids Can Change and Adapt

Development isn't set in stone. Kids are constantly changing and adapting to their environment. Even kids who have had difficult experiences can overcome them and go on to have successful lives.

So, when researchers are looking at data from the ABCD Study, they shouldn't assume that a child's current situation will determine their future. They need to remember that all kids have the potential to change and grow.

Best Practices for Using Developmental Data

Here are some tips for researchers to use developmental data responsibly:

  • Be respectful of the kids and families who are participating in the research.

  • Make sure to understand the different life experiences of the kids in the study.

  • Use multiple variables to measure different aspects of kids' lives.

  • Don't forget that unmeasured factors can also influence development.

  • Avoid using language that suggests that kids' futures are predetermined.

  • Be aware of how findings could be misinterpreted.

By following these tips, everyone can use developmental data to help improve the lives of all young people.

Link to Article

Abstract

The number and size of open-access developmental data sets that include brain and behavioral information have dramatically increased in recent years (Fig. 1; see also Table S1 in the Supplemental Material). These collaborative initiatives represent a new era of science that democratizes data access, facilitates scientific discovery, boosts statistical power, enhances reproducibility and replication, and can inform policy (Rosenberg et al., 2018). However, to use these data responsibly, we must consider the broader social context, the dynamic and interactive process of development, and the strength and limitations of the data when formulating our research questions, designing statistical models, and interpreting our findings. Here, we use the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, the largest longitudinal study of brain development and youth health in the United States to date, to suggest best practices for responsible data use. To be developmentally informed and responsible users of these data, we must consider (a) the heterogeneity of experiences within the broader social context in which development occurs and (b) the potential for adaptation to the environment and developmental change.

Understanding How Kids Grow and Change: A Guide to Using the ABCD Study

The Importance of Different Experiences

The ABCD Study is a huge project that's following thousands of kids as they grow up. It's giving scientists a lot of information about how kids' brains, bodies, and lives change over time. But it's important to remember that every kid is different and has their own unique experiences.

The study doesn't include everyone, and it can't measure everything that might affect a kid's development. For example, it doesn't have information about things like racism or poverty, which can have a big impact on kids' lives. So, when scientists use the ABCD Study to learn about kids, they need to keep in mind that there are other things going on that they might not know about.

How Kids Change and Adapt

Growing up is a journey, and kids are constantly changing and adapting to their environment. Sometimes, things that might seem bad in one situation can actually be helpful in another. For example, a kid who experiences some challenges early in life might develop skills that help them succeed later on.

It's also important to remember that kids' brains are still developing, and they can change a lot over time. Just because something is true for a kid at one point in their life doesn't mean it will always be true.

Using the ABCD Study Responsibly

Here are some tips for using the ABCD Study responsibly:

  • Remember that every kid is an individual.

  • Think about the different experiences that kids might have.

  • Be careful not to jump to conclusions based on just one point in time.

  • Recognize that kids can change and grow.

  • Be aware of how your findings might be used to help or harm kids.

By following these tips, everyone can use the ABCD Study to learn more about how kids develop and to create a better future for all kids.

Link to Article

Abstract

The number and size of open-access developmental data sets that include brain and behavioral information have dramatically increased in recent years (Fig. 1; see also Table S1 in the Supplemental Material). These collaborative initiatives represent a new era of science that democratizes data access, facilitates scientific discovery, boosts statistical power, enhances reproducibility and replication, and can inform policy (Rosenberg et al., 2018). However, to use these data responsibly, we must consider the broader social context, the dynamic and interactive process of development, and the strength and limitations of the data when formulating our research questions, designing statistical models, and interpreting our findings. Here, we use the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, the largest longitudinal study of brain development and youth health in the United States to date, to suggest best practices for responsible data use. To be developmentally informed and responsible users of these data, we must consider (a) the heterogeneity of experiences within the broader social context in which development occurs and (b) the potential for adaptation to the environment and developmental change.

Being Smart and Fair with Big Data about Kids

The Different Lives of Kids

The ABCD Study is a huge project that's following thousands of kids as they grow up. It tells scientists about their lives, their brains, and how they're growing up. But it's important to remember that every kid is different. They live in different places, have different families, and go through different things.

For example, some kids live in rich neighborhoods with lots of parks and good schools. Other kids live in poor neighborhoods with not as many safe places to play or good schools to go to. These differences can make a big impact on how kids grow up.

Kids Can Change and Grow

Even though the ABCD Study gives scientists a lot of information about kids, it's just a snapshot of their lives at one point in time. Kids are always changing and growing. Just because something is true for a kid now doesn't mean it will always be true.

For example, a kid who is having trouble in school might get help from a tutor or a special program. That could make a big difference in how they do in school later on.

How to Use the ABCD Study Wisely

When scientists use the ABCD Study to learn about kids, they need to be careful. They need to remember that:

  • Every kid is different.

  • Kids can change and grow.

  • We don't know everything about how kids develop.

If scientists keep these things in mind, they can use the ABCD Study to help understand kids better and make a difference in their lives.

Link to Article

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Simmons, C. et al. (2021). Responsible Use of Open-Access Developmental Data: The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Psychological Science, 32(6), 866–870. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567976211003564

    Highlights