Psychosocial Stress Reversibly Disrupts Prefrontal Processing and Attentional Control
C. Liston
B.S. McEwen
B.J. Casey
SummaryOriginal

Summary

Stress impairs brain function in attention, memory, and decision-making, but these effects are reversed when stress is reduced, restoring cognitive abilities to pre-stress levels.

2009

Psychosocial Stress Reversibly Disrupts Prefrontal Processing and Attentional Control

Keywords Stress; prefrontal cortex; brain scans; fMRI; attention span; experiment

Abstract

Relatively little is known about the long-term neurobiological sequelae of chronic stress, which predisposes susceptible patients to neuropsychiatric conditions affecting the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Animal models and human neuroimaging experiments provide complementary insights, yet efforts to integrate the two are often complicated by limitations inherent in drawing comparisons between unrelated studies with disparate designs. Translating from a rodent model of chronic stress where we have shown reversible disruption of PFC function, we show that psychosocial stress induces long-lasting but reversible impairments in behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures of PFC function in humans. Twenty healthy adults, exposed to 1 month of psychosocial stress, confirmed by a validated rating scale, were scanned while performing a PFC-dependent attention-shifting task. One month later, they returned for a second scanning session after a period of reduced stress, and their performance was compared with a twice-scanned, matched group of low-stress controls. Psychosocial stress selectively impaired attentional control and disrupted functional connectivity within a frontoparietal network that mediates attention shifts. These effects were reversible: after one month of reduced stress, the same subjects showed no significant differences from controls. These results highlight the plasticity of PFC networks in healthy human subjects and suggest one mechanism by which disrupted plasticity may contribute to cognitive impairments characteristic of stress-related neuropsychiatric conditions in susceptible individuals.

Chronic stress is a well-known risk factor for several major neuropsychiatric conditions that affect the prefrontal cortex (PFC), including depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and anxiety disorders (17). In healthy subjects, it disrupts creativity, flexible problem solving, working memory, and other PFC-dependent processes (810). Efforts to investigate the neurobiological basis of these associations are hampered by our limited understanding of the long-term effects of stress on the PFC. A variety of external stress treatments and monoaminergic pharmacologic manipulations have been shown to alter PFC function acutely in rats, monkeys, and human subjects, acting on a timescale of seconds to minutes to increase monoaminergic tone above optimal levels (8, 9). In contrast, relatively little is known about how naturalistic, chronic psychosocial stressors affect PFC function in the long term.

Functional neuroimaging studies are a powerful tool for assessing PFC function in human subjects, but those studies may be mechanistically less informative in the absence of results from animal models that can be used to constrain hypotheses and data interpretation. Recent studies in rats have begun to address this issue. They show that 21 days of repeated restraint stress reduce dendritic arborization and spine density in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), decreasing axospinous inputs to pyramidal cells by as much as 33% (1113). These structural changes have significant functional consequences: chronic stress selectively impairs attentional set-shifting, which depends on mPFC integrity, but not reversal learning, a cognitive function of comparable difficulty that is independent of the mPFC (1416). Importantly, chronic stress effects on the rodent mPFC are reversible 4 weeks after cessation of the stressor, a finding that highlights both the plasticity and the resilience of mPFC pyramidal cells (17).

This study was designed to assess whether comparable changes are detectable in human subjects performing an attentional set-shifting task designed to capture key features of the rodent paradigm, while functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) gauged PFC integrity. Twenty healthy young adults were tested after 4 weeks of psychosocial stress exposure as they prepared for a major academic examination, and their performance was compared with 20 control subjects matched for age, gender, and occupation. Stress exposure was confirmed and quantified using the 10-item Cohen perceived stress scale (PSS), a well-validated questionnaire that gauges chronic stress on a 40-point scale (18, 19) and has been used successfully in related work (20, 21). Finally, the same subjects returned after 4 weeks of reduced stress and were reassessed relative to matched controls with equal task experience, thus yielding an assessment of the reversibility of stress effects on PFC function while controlling for unidentified group differences, selection biases, or other confounding variables.

Results

Chronic Psychosocial Stress Selectively Impaired Attention Shifts.

All subjects were trained and tested on a visual discrimination task that yielded dissociable measures of attention shifts and response reversals. The task design and validity are described in detail elsewhere [see supporting information ( and ref. 22]. Briefly, subjects viewed two circular square-wave gratings on each trial. The gratings were either red or green and moved either up or down, and the stimuli varied independently along these two dimensions. A centrally located cue (“M” or “C”) instructed subjects to respond to either the motion of the stimuli (select the upward-moving grating) or their color (select the red grating) while ignoring the other dimension. Attention shifts were assessed by contrasting performance on shift trials—defined as those preceded by 2–5 trials of the opposite dimension—with repeat trials, which were preceded by 2–5 trials of the same dimension (Fig. 1A). Response reversals were assessed by contrasting shift trials that required a reversal of the prepotent response learned in the previous block of repeats with those that did not (Fig. 1B). Previous work showed that response reversals are comparably difficult but are mediated by a network independent of the prefrontal areas that mediate attention shifts (22), in analogy to the task paradigm used in the rodent model (15).

zpq9990961610001

Fig. 1. Chronic psychosocial stress selectively impaired attention shifting. (A) Attention-shift paradigm. Subjects viewed two moving, circular square-wave gratings on each trial and were cued to respond on the basis of either the color (“C”) or the motion (“M”) of the stimuli. Attention shifting was assessed by contrasting shift trials—defined as those preceded by 2–5 trials of the opposite dimension—with repeat trials, which were preceded by 2–5 trials of the same dimension but were otherwise identical. (B) Reversal learning paradigm. On some shift trials (“reversals”), the target response for the color dimension (red) was paired with the nontarget for the motion dimension (down), so the subject was required to override the response learned in the previous block of repeats. On others, the target response was the same in both dimensions. Response reversals were assessed by contrasting shift trials that required a reversal of the prepotent response learned in the previous block of repeats with those that did not. (C) Psychosocial stress impaired attention shifts. Across subjects, PSS scores predicted larger attention shift costs (r = 0.51, P = 0.002). (D) Stress effects on attention shifts were specific: PSS scores were not associated with reversal costs (r = 0.10, P = 0.56).

Consistent with previous work (22), shift trials were slower than repeat trials (t = 33.23, P < 0.001). This attention-shifting cost [mean shift response time (RT) − mean repeat RT] was elevated in chronically stressed subjects, (t = 2.10, P = 0.04), who reported significantly higher PSS scores (t = 4.51, P < 0.001). Across subjects in both groups, individual PSS scores predicted impairments in attention shifts (Fig. 1C: r = 0.51, P = 0.002) but not in response reversals (Fig. 1D: r = 0.10, P = 0.56). This effect did not reflect a general impairment in speed of processing, since repeat trial reaction time was not significantly elevated in stressed subjects (t = 1.59, P = 0.12). Moreover, response reversal performance in the two groups was equivalent (t = 0.64, P = 0.53), indicating that stress selectively disrupted attention shifts and not other processes of comparable difficulty. The association between stress and attention shifting was not confounded by sleep habits. Subjects in the two groups reported sleeping for an equivalent length of time during the night preceding the testing session (t = 0.86, P = 0.40), and across both groups there was no correlation between sleep and attention-shifting costs (r = 0.12, P = 0.46).

Chronic Psychosocial Stress Disrupted Prefrontal Functional Connectivity.

Functional imaging data confirmed that attention shifts engaged a frontoparietal network that included dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) (P < 0.05, corrected; Fig. 2A), a putative homolog of the rodent dorsal mPFC (23). Studies in rats suggest that attention-shifting impairments may be due in part to chronic stress-related decreases in axospinous inputs to the apical dendrites of prefrontal layer II/III pyramidal cells in this area (15). These dendrites are the target of long-range corticocortical projections and are assumed to play an important computational role in cognitive functions mediated by a distributed network of structures (24). Accordingly, we reasoned that if chronic stress reduces long-range corticocortical axospinous input to the PFC by reducing dendritic arborization, then it may also disrupt fMRI measures of corticocortical connectivity.

zpq9990961610002

Fig. 2. Flexible attentional control depends on the integrity of a frontoparietal network that includes DLPFC. (A) Attention shifts engaged DLPFC bilaterally (P < 0.05, corrected). (B) The areas depicted in A served as seed volumes for a functional connectivity analysis that quantified coupling between DLPFC and other areas of a frontoparietal network that was active during attention shifts, including anterior cingulate (ACC), ventrolateral prefrontal (VLPFC), insula, premotor, ventral and dorsal areas of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), and occipitotemporal visual areas including the fusiform cortex (P < 0.05, corrected). (C) Attention-shift performance depended on the integrity of this network. Decreased functional connectivity between left (i) and right (ii) DLPFC and areas of posterior parietal and premotor cortex was correlated with impaired attention shifting, independent of stress effects (P < 0.05, corrected). Scatterplots depict results for peak voxels in each cluster. See SI for details. ***, P < 0.005.

To test this hypothesis, we used functional connectivity analysis to assess whether stress exposure modulated DLPFC coupling with other areas of the attention network (see refs. 25 and 26 and SI for details). This analysis quantified coupling between DLPFC and other areas of a frontoparietal antentional network, including anterior cingulate, ventrolateral prefrontal, insula, premotor, posterior parietal, and occipitotemporal visual areas (Fig. 2B), while controlling for variation in the magnitude of task-related activity. To assess whether attention-shifting performance depends on the integrity of this network, we performed a multivariate linear regression of shift cost on measures of prefrontal connectivity, while controlling for stress (PSS scores) as a covariate. Decreased functional coupling between DLPFC and areas of premotor and posterior parietal cortex was associated with greater impairments in attention shifting (Fig. 2C). This finding shows that effective attention shifting depends in part on the integrity of the frontoparietal network shown in Fig. 2B, suggesting that disrupted connectivity may impair attention shifts.

Next, we used multifactorial ANOVA to assess whether psychosocial stress may cause disruptions of this type (Fig. 3A). Chronically stressed subjects showed a relative decoupling of left DLPFC with areas of right DLPFC, left premotor, bilateral ventral PFC, and left posterior parietal cortex, whereas stress increased coupling with temporal lobe areas devoted to visual processing. Analysis of right DLPFC coupling showed decreased connectivity with left DLPFC, right ventral PFC, striatum, right premotor cortex, cingulate cortex, left fusiform cortex, and left cerebellum. Together, these results support the hypothesis that attention shifting depends in part on the integrity of a frontoparietal network that includes DLPFC and that stress-related impairments in DLPFC connectivity may contribute to a decline in flexible attentional control. (See SI for further analytical and statistical details.)

zpq9990961610003

Fig. 3. Chronic stress reversibly disrupted DLPFC functional connectivity. (A) Psychosocial stress exposure in the month preceding the first scanning session was associated with altered functional connectivity in left and right DLPFC. (Left) Stress decreased coupling between left DLPFC and right DLPFC, premotor, ventral PFC (insula), and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) relative to controls. Stress increased coupling with middle temporal lobe areas. (Right) Psychosocial stress decreased coupling between right DLPFC and left DLPFC, anterior cingulate (ACC), premotor, ventral PFC (insula), putamen, ventral PPC, posterior cingulate (PCC), fusiform cortex, and cerebellum. Maps represent post hoc t-tests of the stress effect. (B) Stress-exposed subjects were retested after 1 month of reduced stress and showed no differences from control subjects on PSS scores (i: t = 0.88, P = 0.39) or attention-shift costs (ii: t = 0.05, P = 0.96). (C) Stress effects on left (i) and right (ii) DLPFC functional connectivity reversed after 1 month of reduced stress for all areas tested except ventrolateral PFC. This reversal was confirmed by a significant stress-by-session interaction for all areas within a search volume that included voxels showing a significant main effect of stress overall. Other areas showed a comparable trend: stressed subjects showed altered connectivity in session 1 (red bars) but not in session 2 (blue bars). Data are plotted relative to mean values in low-stress control subjects. Error bars, SEM; NS, not significant; , interaction significant at P < 0.05; t-tests, *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; ***, P < 0.005.

Stress Effects on Attention and Prefrontal Connectivity Were Reversible.

In combination with data from rodent models, the data suggest that chronic psychosocial stress effects on attention shifting may be related to alterations in functional properties of the PFC, which may in turn reflect disruptions in dendritic arborization and axospinous input to this region of the type observed in rats. However, those effects may instead reflect other unidentified confounding variables, whereby subjects with impaired prefrontal processing at baseline may also perceive situations and events to be more stressful. Conversely, if psychosocial stress truly does impair PFC function in the long term, it may be clinically useful to know whether these effects are reversible, as they are in rodent models after 1 month of reduced stress (17).

To address these two issues, chronically stressed subjects returned for a second scanning session ≈4 weeks after cessation of the stressor (1 month after their examination date). They were retested on the perceived stress scale to confirm a reduction in psychosocial stress during the interim. Stress-exposed subjects who reported persistently elevated PSS scores in session 2, defined as 1 standard deviation above the normative population mean, were excluded. To control for practice effects, we retested an equal number of control subjects, excluding those whose PSS scores on retest exceeded the normative population mean by 1 standard deviation. (See SI for additional details.)

After 1 month of reduced stress, high-stress subjects from the first session showed no significant differences from the constant, low-stress controls on measures of perceived stress or attention shifting (Fig. 3B). Three-factor (stress, session, subject), mixed-effects ANOVA was used to identify regions showing an effect of stress in session 1, but not session 2, confirmed by a significant session-by-stress interaction. The search volume for this ANOVA comprised all areas showing a significant main effect of stress overall such that the selection of voxels to be analyzed was independent of the interaction being tested. This ensured that the reversibility analysis was not biased by the results obtained in session 1. Stress effects on DLPFC functional coupling reversed in all areas included in the search volume except ventral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann Area 13/47), which remained decoupled relative to controls (Fig. 3C). Areas showing smaller effects in session one—including cingulate, posterior parietal, and higher-order visual areas—did not meet search volume criteria so the significance of a reversal interaction could not be confirmed. However, t-tests of connectivity in these areas revealed the same trend: significantly disrupted connectivity in session 1 but not in session 2 (Fig. 3C). These data suggest that chronic psychosocial stress and not some other confounding variable impairs PFC processing and that these impairments are largely reversible. They also suggest that the effects of stress on PFC function observed here reflect a predominantly state-dependent phenomenon, rather than a trait-dependent one.

Discussion

Collectively, our results demonstrate that psychosocial stress induces changes in human PFC function that persist in the absence of any acute stressor and support the utility of the rodent repeated restraint paradigm for modeling aspects of human PFC plasticity in states of chronic stress. Fig. 4 highlights data from our rodent model and the parallel findings observed here. In both studies, chronic stress disrupted attention shifting (Fig. 4A) and PFC circuitry (Fig. 4B), and measures of PFC integrity predicted attention-shifting performance (Fig. 4C).

zpq9990961610004

Fig. 4. Stress effects on human PFC function (Bottom) are consistent with those observed in a rodent model of chronic stress (Top). Data from the rodent model are reproduced with permission from ref. 15 (Copyright 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience). (A) Chronic stress disrupted DLPFC functional connectivity in human subjects (t = 5.74, P < 0.001) and reduces apical dendritic arborization in rats (t = 2.83, P = 0.007). Human functional connectivity values represent the group means for peak voxels in each of the affected regions depicted in Fig. 3 A and C. (B) Stress-induced corresponding impairments in attention shifting [humans (Bottom), t = 2.10, P = 0.04; rats (Top), t = 3.51, P = 0.002). (C) Measures of PFC integrity predicted attention-shifting impairments in humans (Bottom) (r = −0.64, P < 0.001) and showed a similar trend in rats (Top) (r = −0.74, P = 0.09). Human functional connectivity values represent the means for peak voxels in each of the 6 regions depicted in the scatterplots in Fig. 2C. Error bars, SEM; *, P < 0.05; **, P < 0.01; ***, P < 0.005.

These findings are informative but should be considered in light of several caveats. First, while rodent studies may be useful for constraining data interpretation in neuroimaging experiments, there are limitations in translating between rodents and human subjects. Most importantly, humans' perception of a naturalistic psychosocial stressor differs qualitatively from a rat's response to repeated restraint in an artificial laboratory environment. Likewise, our data confirm that there is considerable variability within human subjects in the degree to which the same trigger—preparing for an academic examination—is perceived to be stressful. Humans evaluate the significance of a psychosocial stressor, and it is this higher-order neocortical processing of the perceived meaning of a stimulus, in conjunction with activity in the medial and central nuclei of the amygdala, that is thought to activate the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the stress response (2729). Thus, it is probable that variability in this factor may have important mechanistic consequences, even if the end points in both rats and humans—structural and functional remodeling of PFC networks—share certain characteristics. Additional research would be required to address that issue.

A second caveat concerns the degree to which our findings can be generalized to other human subjects. We opted to study medical students preparing for a major examination because the exposure was perceived to be highly stressful for a period of weeks but also came with a preset limit on its duration, followed by a period of reduced stress, thus permitting a within-subjects control and an assessment of reversibility. One prospective study examined the relation between PSS scores and hippocampal volumes in postmenopausal women and found a reduction in hippocampal gray matter volumes (21), suggesting that stress effects on human brain structure may generalize to other contexts. Still, future studies should replicate these results in other populations under other forms of stress.

A third caveat concerns the benefits and limitations of the PSS scale for quantifying stress exposure. The PSS scale is an extensively validated tool designed to quantify subjects' experience of psychosocial stress and trace changes in stress exposure over time (18, 21), but it does not assess stress-related physiological changes. Other studies have followed diurnal measurements of salivary cortisol for this purpose (3032). They strongly implicate glucocorticoids in the brain's response to stress, although it is likely that they play a permissive role in a cascade that also involves excitatory amino acids, neurotrophins, adhesion molecules, altered glucocorticoid receptor expression patterns, and neuromodulators like serotonin (33). Accordingly, diurnal cortisol is a complex end point for gauging chronic psychosocial stress, which the PSS assesses directly. (See SI for more detailed discussion.)

Even in light of these limitations, our results are provocative and provide several potentially useful insights. First, they show that chronic, psychosocial stress selectively and reversibly disrupts human PFC function. Second, they demonstrate the utility of translating from animal data to constrain hypotheses in human neuroimaging work and support the validity of the rodent repeated restraint stress model for elucidating mechanistic aspects of the response to chronic stress in humans. Chronic stress disrupted coupling within a frontoparietal attentional network in human subjects in a manner that can be easily understood within the framework of rodent studies showing alterations in dendritic arborization and axospinous inputs, which in turn may disrupt both local oscillatory activity within the PFC and long-range corticocortical connections between the PFC and more distant areas. These changes, in turn, may interfere with top—down regulation of activity by the DLPFC and interfere with functional coupling. The DLPFC, acting in concert with a network of higher-order association areas, is believed to regulate attention directly by biasing processing in occipitotemporal visual cortex, favoring one dimension over another (34, 35). Anterior cingulate and posterior parietal cortex act to detect conflicts in information processing (e.g., color vs. motion processing) and signal to the DLPFC the need for increased top—down control (22). In this view, functional coupling within the network would be important for both regulating DLPFC activity and mediating its effects (24), an interpretation supported by the correlations with attention-shifting performance depicted in Fig. 2C.

Third, they add to a complementary body of literature that has elucidated the mechanisms by which stress alters PFC function acutely. Dopamine and norepinephrine enhance PFC function via D1 and α2 adrenergic receptor stimulation, which in turn enhances functional connectivity within PFC networks by inhibiting cAMP-dependent hyperpolarization (8, 36). In contrast, excessive monoamine release associated with acute uncontrollable stressors has just the opposite effect, impairing PFC-dependent working memory and cognitive flexibility in a manner preventable by pretreatment with pharmacologic antagonists of these neuromodulators (8, 10). Other studies indicate that acute alterations in neuromodulatory systems affect attention shifting as well (37). The results presented here complement these studies by identifying long-term but reversible prefrontal functional impairments in chronically stressed human subjects even in the absence of any acute stress treatment. Stress effects on PFC function may therefore be attributable to both acute changes in monoaminergic tone and longer-term structural changes in dendritic arborization and spine density that perturb functional connectivity (12, 15). Of course, additional work in both rodents and human subjects will be required to confirm an association between structural plasticity in the former and functional connectivity in the latter and to establish a causal relationship between these factors and cognitive impairments.

Finally, our results may prove to be clinically informative. The diagnostic criteria for major depression, generalized anxiety disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, and other stress-related neuropsychiatric conditions include deficits in the cognitive control of attention (38). Our results show that stress induces comparable deficits in healthy human subjects as well, a finding consistent with previous work linking PSS scores with measures of executive function (20) and hippocampal volume (21) in nonclinical populations. Thus, they highlight the therapeutic potential for stress-reduction interventions by delineating 1 neural mechanism by which chronic stress may cause these cognitive deficits.

In some contexts, stress-induced plasticity may be beneficial. In rodents, for example, it may serve a neuroprotective function by reducing excitatory neurotoxicity in the hippocampus (33). It is also notable that prefrontal connectivity did not decrease uniformly: stress increased DLPFC coupling with temporal lobe areas that contribute to visual processing at the expense of association areas mediating flexibility and control. These differences may be due in part to other unidentified changes in frontoparietal attentional circuitry and to selective pruning of some connections but not others. They are consistent with the view that stress effects on prefrontal connectivity may be adaptive in the short term, to the extent that they bias processing in favor of a single, salient stimulus, in a manner that reverses after a period of reduced stress in healthy individuals. When susceptible individuals are exposed to repeated, chronic stress, by contrast, impairments in PFC-mediated flexibility may persist, counteracting short-term benefits in a way that may ultimately contribute to the diverse symptomatology of chronic stress-related neuropsychiatric diseases (2, 39).

Methods

Subjects.

The remaining 40 subjects (20 stressed and 20 controls matched for age, sex, and sleep habits) participated in an initial scanning session that included 3 components: (1) the Cohen perceived stress scale, (2) attentional control task training, and (3) a fMRI scan while being tested on the same task. These are described below, with additional details available as SI. All subjects were asked to return for a second session, ≈1 month after the first, and retested on the same measures. The experimental procedure was approved by the Weill Cornell Medical College Institutional Review Board, and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects before scanning.

Perceived Stress Scale.

Stress was quantified by self-report at the start of each session using the Cohen PSS, a standardized and reliable measure of an individual's perception of chronic psychosocial stress (18, 19).

Attentional Control Task.

The task was as described above (Fig. 1) and in more detail elsewhere (22). On each trial, subjects responded manually by pressing a button with their right (dominant) hand corresponding to the target stimulus as described in SI. Color and motion trials were counterbalanced for trial type, dimension, and side of target presentation. Before each scanning session, subjects were trained on 3 blocks of 36 trials consisting of color discriminations, motion discriminations, and alternating color/motion discriminations, respectively. In the scanner, subjects completed 6 blocks of 72 trials, which were presented in a jittered task design.

Functional MRI Analysis.

Functional MR images were acquired on a GE 3T scanner using a spiral in-and-out sequence (40) while subjects performed this task. MR images were preprocessed and analyzed using the AFNI software package (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov). This analysis was designed to assess whether psychosocial stress modulated DLPFC functional connectivity and included four steps. First, a general linear model and mixed-effects ANOVA were used to identify areas of DLPFC that were more active during shift trials than during repeat trials. Attention shifting was found to engage DLPFC bilaterally. Second, these two areas—left and right DLPFC (Brodmann Area 8/9)—served as seed volumes for functional connectivity analyses (25, 26) that delineated a frontoparietal network coupled to DLPFC, while controlling for global fluctuations in the MR signal and the magnitude of task-dependent activity. Third, to assess whether functional coupling within this network was important for attention shifting independent of stress effects, we performed a multivariate linear regression of shift cost on measures of prefrontal connectivity, while controlling for stress (PSS scores) as a covariate. Fourth, we examined how functional connectivity varied with stress, using a two-factor mixed-effects ANOVA and voxelwise t-tests of connectivity (R2) in stressed vs. control subjects.

Reversibility Analysis.

To control for confounding variables unrelated to stress and to assess the reversibility of stress effects on PFC function, 15 stress-exposed subjects were rescanned after 1 month of reduced stress. To control for practice effects, we retested an equal number of control subjects. Three-factor (stress, high vs. low; session, 1 vs. 2; and subject), mixed-effects ANOVA, and post hoc t-tests (session 1, high vs. low stress; session 2, high vs. low stress) were used to assess reversibility of stress effects on PSS scores, shift costs, and functional connectivity. Stress-by-session interactions were used to confirm the reversibility of connectivity effects within a search volume that included all areas showing a main effect of stress, averaged over the first and second sessions, so that the analysis region was independent of the interaction tested. This search volume excluded several regions showing smaller effects in session 1. To examine whether these areas followed a similar trend, we performed simple t-tests on the peak voxel in each cluster.

Link to Article

Abstract

Relatively little is known about the long-term neurobiological sequelae of chronic stress, which predisposes susceptible patients to neuropsychiatric conditions affecting the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Animal models and human neuroimaging experiments provide complementary insights, yet efforts to integrate the two are often complicated by limitations inherent in drawing comparisons between unrelated studies with disparate designs. Translating from a rodent model of chronic stress where we have shown reversible disruption of PFC function, we show that psychosocial stress induces long-lasting but reversible impairments in behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures of PFC function in humans. Twenty healthy adults, exposed to 1 month of psychosocial stress, confirmed by a validated rating scale, were scanned while performing a PFC-dependent attention-shifting task. One month later, they returned for a second scanning session after a period of reduced stress, and their performance was compared with a twice-scanned, matched group of low-stress controls. Psychosocial stress selectively impaired attentional control and disrupted functional connectivity within a frontoparietal network that mediates attention shifts. These effects were reversible: after one month of reduced stress, the same subjects showed no significant differences from controls. These results highlight the plasticity of PFC networks in healthy human subjects and suggest one mechanism by which disrupted plasticity may contribute to cognitive impairments characteristic of stress-related neuropsychiatric conditions in susceptible individuals.

Chronic Psychosocial Stress Disrupts Functional Connectivity in the Human Brain

Chronic stress is a well-established risk factor for various neuropsychiatric disorders, including depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and anxiety disorders, all of which impact prefrontal cortex (PFC) function. Healthy individuals experiencing chronic stress exhibit impaired creativity, problem-solving abilities, working memory, and other PFC-dependent cognitive processes. Despite its significance, the long-term effects of stress on the PFC remain poorly understood, hindering investigations into the neurobiological underpinnings of these associations.

While short-term stress responses and their effects on PFC function have been studied through pharmacological manipulation and external stressors in humans and animal models, the impact of naturalistic, chronic psychosocial stressors on PFC function over extended periods is less clear. Functional neuroimaging techniques offer valuable insights into human PFC function, but their mechanistic interpretations benefit significantly from animal models, which can provide crucial context and constrain hypotheses. Recent rodent studies have started addressing this gap, revealing that 21 days of repeated restraint stress lead to reduced dendritic arborization and spine density in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). This results in a substantial decrease (up to 33%) in axospinous inputs to pyramidal cells. Notably, these structural changes have significant functional implications, selectively impairing attentional set-shifting, a cognitive function reliant on mPFC integrity, while leaving reversal learning, another cognitive function of comparable complexity that is independent of the mPFC, unaffected. Significantly, the effects of chronic stress on the rodent mPFC are reversible within four weeks of stressor cessation, highlighting the plasticity and resilience of mPFC pyramidal cells.

This study aimed to investigate if similar changes are observable in humans performing an attentional set-shifting task, employing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to evaluate PFC integrity. Twenty healthy young adults were assessed after a four-week period of psychosocial stress induced by preparations for a major academic examination. Their performance was compared to a control group of 20 individuals matched for age, gender, and occupation. The 10-item Cohen Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), a validated questionnaire measuring chronic stress on a 40-point scale, was used to confirm and quantify stress exposure. To evaluate the reversibility of stress effects on PFC function, the same subjects were reassessed after a four-week period of reduced stress and compared to matched controls with equivalent task experience. This approach helped control for potential group differences, selection biases, or other confounding variables.

Results

Chronic Psychosocial Stress Selectively Impairs Attention Shifts.

A visual discrimination task was used to assess all subjects, yielding distinct measures of attention shifts and response reversals. The task design and validation have been previously described. In brief, subjects viewed two circular square-wave gratings per trial, varying independently in color (red or green) and motion (up or down). A central cue instructed subjects to respond to either the motion or color of the stimuli, ignoring the other dimension. Attention shifts were assessed by comparing performance on shift trials (preceded by 2–5 trials of the opposite dimension) with repeat trials (preceded by 2–5 trials of the same dimension; Figure 1A). Response reversals were evaluated by contrasting shift trials requiring a reversal of the prepotent response from the previous block of repeats with those that did not (Figure 1B). As demonstrated in previous studies, response reversals, while similarly challenging, are mediated by a network distinct from the prefrontal areas responsible for attention shifts.

Consistent with previous findings, shift trials elicited slower responses compared to repeat trials (t = 33.23, P < 0.001). This attention-shifting cost (mean shift response time [RT] - mean repeat RT) was amplified in the chronically stressed group (t = 2.10, P = 0.04), who also reported significantly higher PSS scores (t = 4.51, P < 0.001). Across both groups, individual PSS scores correlated with impairments in attention shifts (Figure 1C: r = 0.51, P = 0.002) but not response reversals (Figure 1D: r = 0.10, P = 0.56). This effect was not indicative of a general decline in processing speed, as repeat trial reaction time did not differ significantly between stressed and control subjects (t = 1.59, P = 0.12). Furthermore, both groups demonstrated comparable response reversal performance (t = 0.64, P = 0.53), suggesting that stress specifically impacted attention shifts rather than other cognitive processes of comparable difficulty. Sleep habits did not confound the association between stress and attention shifting. Both groups reported similar sleep durations the night before testing (t = 0.86, P = 0.40), and no correlation was observed between sleep and attention-shifting costs across all subjects (r = 0.12, P = 0.46).

Chronic Psychosocial Stress Disrupts Prefrontal Functional Connectivity.

Functional imaging data corroborated that attention shifts engaged a frontoparietal network, including the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) (P < 0.05, corrected; Figure 2A), believed to be a homolog of the rodent dorsal mPFC. Studies in rats suggest that impairments in attention shifting due to chronic stress might stem partly from diminished axospinous input to the apical dendrites of layer II/III pyramidal cells within the PFC. These dendrites receive long-range corticocortical projections and are thought to play a crucial computational role in cognitive functions mediated by a distributed network of brain regions. Therefore, we hypothesized that if chronic stress weakens long-range corticocortical axospinous input to the PFC by reducing dendritic arborization, it might also affect fMRI measures of corticocortical connectivity.

To test this hypothesis, we employed functional connectivity analysis to examine whether stress exposure modulated DLPFC coupling with other regions within the attention network (see SI for detailed methodology). This analysis quantified the strength of coupling between the DLPFC and other areas within the frontoparietal attention network, including the anterior cingulate, ventrolateral prefrontal, insula, premotor, posterior parietal, and occipitotemporal visual areas (Figure 2B), while controlling for variations in the magnitude of task-related activity. To determine if attention-shifting performance relies on the integrity of this network, we conducted a multivariate linear regression analysis with shift cost as the dependent variable and measures of prefrontal connectivity as predictors, while controlling for stress (PSS scores) as a covariate. Diminished functional coupling between DLPFC and areas of the premotor and posterior parietal cortex was associated with greater impairment in attention shifting (Figure 2C). This result suggests that efficient attention shifting partially depends on the integrity of the frontoparietal network depicted in Figure 2B, implying that disruptions in connectivity may contribute to impaired attention shifts.

Next, a multifactorial ANOVA was used to investigate whether psychosocial stress might induce such disruptions (Figure 3A). Compared to controls, chronically stressed subjects exhibited a relative decoupling of the left DLPFC with regions of the right DLPFC, left premotor cortex, bilateral ventral PFC, and left posterior parietal cortex. Conversely, stress amplified coupling with temporal lobe regions involved in visual processing. Analysis of right DLPFC coupling revealed decreased connectivity with the left DLPFC, right ventral PFC, striatum, right premotor cortex, cingulate cortex, left fusiform cortex, and left cerebellum. Taken together, these findings support the hypothesis that attention shifting relies partly on the integrity of a frontoparietal network encompassing the DLPFC, and that stress-related impairments in DLPFC connectivity might contribute to a decline in flexible attentional control. (Refer to SI for further analytical and statistical details.)

Stress Effects on Attention and Prefrontal Connectivity Were Reversible.

Converging evidence from rodent models and our data suggests that the effects of chronic psychosocial stress on attention shifting might be linked to alterations in PFC functional properties. These alterations, in turn, could reflect disruptions in dendritic arborization and axospinous input to this brain region, similar to observations in rats. However, these effects could also be attributed to other unidentified confounding variables, such as individuals with pre-existing impaired prefrontal processing perceiving situations and events as more stressful. Conversely, if psychosocial stress indeed impairs PFC function over the long term, it would be clinically relevant to understand whether these effects are reversible, as observed in rodent models after a month of reduced stress.

To address these two possibilities, the chronically stressed subjects participated in a second scanning session approximately four weeks after the cessation of the stressor (one month after their examination date). They were re-evaluated using the perceived stress scale to confirm a reduction in psychosocial stress during the intervening period. Stress-exposed individuals who continued to exhibit elevated PSS scores in session 2 (defined as one standard deviation above the normative population mean) were excluded from further analysis. To account for practice effects, an equal number of control subjects were re-tested, excluding those whose PSS scores on retest exceeded the normative population mean by one standard deviation. (See SI for additional details.)

Following one month of reduced stress, the high-stress subjects from the first session no longer differed significantly from the consistently low-stress controls on measures of perceived stress or attention shifting (Figure 3B). A three-factor (stress, high vs. low; session, 1 vs. 2; and subject) mixed-effects ANOVA was conducted to identify regions displaying a significant effect of stress in session 1 but not in session 2, confirmed by a significant session-by-stress interaction. The search volume for this ANOVA encompassed all areas exhibiting a significant main effect of stress overall, ensuring that the selection of voxels for analysis was independent of the interaction being tested. This approach prevented the reversibility analysis from being biased by the findings from session 1. Stress effects on DLPFC functional coupling reversed in all regions within the search volume except for the ventral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann Area 13/47), which remained decoupled relative to controls (Figure 3C). Regions demonstrating smaller effects in session 1, such as the cingulate, posterior parietal, and higher-order visual areas, did not meet the criteria for inclusion in the search volume, precluding confirmation of a statistically significant reversal interaction. However, t-tests of connectivity in these areas revealed a similar trend: significantly disrupted connectivity in session 1 but not in session 2 (Figure 3C). These data suggest that chronic psychosocial stress, rather than other confounding variables, impairs PFC processing, and that these impairments are largely reversible. They also suggest that the observed stress effects on PFC function primarily reflect a state-dependent phenomenon rather than a trait-dependent one.

Discussion

Our results collectively demonstrate that psychosocial stress triggers alterations in human PFC function that persist even in the absence of an acute stressor. They also underscore the value of leveraging rodent models to inform and refine hypotheses in human neuroimaging studies, supporting the validity of the rodent repeated restraint stress paradigm for investigating mechanistic aspects of chronic stress response in humans. Figure 4 illustrates data from our rodent model alongside the parallel findings from the human study. Both studies demonstrate that chronic stress disrupts attention shifting (Figure 4A) and PFC circuitry (Figure 4B), with measures of PFC integrity predicting attention-shifting performance (Figure 4C).

While these findings are insightful, they should be considered in light of certain limitations. First, despite the usefulness of rodent studies for interpreting human neuroimaging data, translating findings between rodents and humans has inherent limitations. Notably, humans' perception of a naturalistic psychosocial stressor differs qualitatively from a rat's response to repeated restraint in an artificial laboratory setting. Similarly, our data confirms considerable interindividual variability among humans in the perceived stressfulness of the same trigger—preparing for an academic examination. Humans evaluate the significance of a psychosocial stressor, and this higher-order neocortical processing of a stimulus's perceived meaning, alongside activity within the medial and central nuclei of the amygdala, is believed to activate the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the stress response. Therefore, variability in this factor might have important mechanistic implications, even if the ultimate outcomes—structural and functional remodeling of PFC networks—share some similarities between rats and humans. Addressing this issue would necessitate further research.

A second caveat pertains to the generalizability of our findings to other human populations. Medical students preparing for a major examination were chosen for this study due to the high perceived stress level of the event over several weeks, coupled with its defined duration and subsequent period of reduced stress. This approach facilitated a within-subjects control and assessment of reversibility. A prospective study examining the relationship between PSS scores and hippocampal volume in postmenopausal women found a reduction in hippocampal gray matter volume, suggesting that stress effects on human brain structure might generalize to other contexts. Nonetheless, future studies should aim to replicate these findings in diverse populations experiencing different forms of stress.

The third caveat concerns the strengths and limitations of the PSS scale for quantifying stress exposure. While the PSS scale is a extensively validated instrument for quantifying individuals' experiences of psychosocial stress and tracking changes in stress levels over time, it does not capture stress-related physiological changes. Other studies have employed diurnal salivary cortisol measurements for this purpose, providing strong evidence for the involvement of glucocorticoids in the brain's response to stress. However, glucocorticoids likely play a permissive role in a complex cascade that involves excitatory amino acids, neurotrophins, adhesion molecules, altered glucocorticoid receptor expression patterns, and neuromodulators like serotonin. Consequently, diurnal cortisol represents a complex endpoint for gauging chronic psychosocial stress, which the PSS assesses more directly. (See SI for a more detailed discussion.)

Even acknowledging these limitations, our findings are intriguing and offer several potentially valuable insights. Firstly, they demonstrate that chronic psychosocial stress selectively and reversibly disrupts human PFC function. Secondly, they highlight the utility of incorporating animal data to inform hypotheses in human neuroimaging research and support the validity of the rodent repeated restraint stress model for elucidating mechanistic aspects of the chronic stress response in humans. Chronic stress in our study disrupted coupling within a frontoparietal attention network in a manner consistent with rodent studies showing alterations in dendritic arborization and axospinous input. These changes, in turn, might disrupt both local oscillatory activity within the PFC and long-range corticocortical connections between the PFC and more distant areas, potentially interfering with top-down regulation of activity by the DLPFC and impairing functional coupling. The DLPFC, in concert with a network of higher-order association areas, is thought to regulate attention directly by biasing processing in the occipitotemporal visual cortex, favoring one dimension over another. The anterior cingulate and posterior parietal cortex are believed to detect conflicts in information processing (e.g., color vs. motion processing) and signal the DLPFC when heightened top-down control is needed. In this framework, functional coupling within the network would be crucial for both regulating DLPFC activity and mediating its effects, an interpretation supported by the correlations with attention-shifting performance depicted in Figure 2C.

Thirdly, our findings contribute to a growing body of literature elucidating the mechanisms underlying stress-induced alterations in acute PFC function. Dopamine and norepinephrine are known to enhance PFC function through D1 and α2 adrenergic receptor stimulation, which in turn strengthens functional connectivity within PFC networks by inhibiting cAMP-dependent hyperpolarization. Conversely, excessive monoamine release associated with acute uncontrollable stressors has the opposite effect, impairing PFC-dependent working memory and cognitive flexibility in a manner preventable by pre-treatment with pharmacological antagonists of these neuromodulators. Other studies indicate that acute alterations in neuromodulatory systems impact attention shifting as well. Our results complement these studies by identifying long-term, yet reversible, prefrontal functional impairments in chronically stressed individuals, even without exposure to an acute stressor. Thus, stress effects on PFC function might be attributed to both acute changes in monoaminergic tone and longer-term structural alterations in dendritic arborization and spine density that disrupt functional connectivity. Of course, further research in both rodents and humans is necessary to confirm a definitive link between structural plasticity in the former and functional connectivity in the latter and establish a causal relationship between these factors and cognitive impairments.

Finally, our results may have clinical implications. The diagnostic criteria for major depression, generalized anxiety disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, and other stress-related neuropsychiatric conditions include deficits in the cognitive control of attention. Our findings reveal that stress induces similar deficits in healthy individuals, aligning with previous research linking PSS scores to measures of executive function and hippocampal volume in non-clinical populations. Therefore, our study highlights the therapeutic potential of stress-reduction interventions by outlining one neural mechanism by which chronic stress may contribute to these cognitive deficits.

In certain contexts, stress-induced plasticity may confer advantages. For instance, it might serve a neuroprotective function in rodents by reducing excitotoxicity in the hippocampus. It is also worth noting that prefrontal connectivity did not universally decrease: stress increased DLPFC coupling with temporal lobe areas involved in visual processing, but at the expense of association areas mediating flexibility and control. These differences might be partly attributable to other unidentified changes within the frontoparietal attention network and to the selective pruning of specific connections. This pattern aligns with the view that stress-induced alterations in prefrontal connectivity might be adaptive in the short term, to the extent that they prioritize processing of a single, salient stimulus, in a manner that reverses after a period of reduced stress in healthy individuals. However, when susceptible individuals experience repeated, chronic stress, impairments in PFC-mediated flexibility may persist, counteracting short-term benefits and potentially contributing to the diverse symptomatology of chronic stress-related neuropsychiatric disorders.

Methods

Subjects.

Forty subjects (20 stressed and 20 controls matched for age, sex, and sleep habits) participated in an initial scanning session involving three components: (1) the Cohen Perceived Stress Scale, (2) attentional control task training, and (3) fMRI scanning during task performance. All subjects were asked to return for a second session approximately one month later for reassessment using the same measures. The Weill Cornell Medical College Institutional Review Board approved the experimental procedures, and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to scanning.

Perceived Stress Scale.

Stress levels were self-reported at the beginning of each session using the Cohen PSS, a standardized and reliable measure of chronic psychosocial stress perception.

Attentional Control Task.

The task design, described elsewhere, involved subjects responding manually on each trial by pressing a button with their right (dominant) hand corresponding to the target stimulus (see SI for details). Color and motion trials were counterbalanced for trial type, dimension, and target presentation side. Prior to scanning, subjects underwent training on three blocks of 36 trials, each focusing on color discriminations, motion discriminations, and alternating color/motion discriminations, respectively. During scanning, subjects completed six blocks of 72 trials presented in a jittered task design.

Functional MRI Analysis.

Functional MR images were acquired using a GE 3T scanner with a spiral in-and-out sequence while subjects performed the task. Image preprocessing and analysis were performed using the AFNI software package (http://afni.nimh.nih.gov). The analysis aimed to determine if psychosocial stress modulated DLPFC functional connectivity and involved four steps. First, a general linear model and mixed-effects ANOVA were used to identify DLPFC regions exhibiting greater activation during shift trials compared to repeat trials, revealing bilateral DLPFC engagement during attention shifting. Second, these two regions, left and right DLPFC (Brodmann Area 8/9), served as seed volumes for functional connectivity analyses that delineated a frontoparietal network coupled to the DLPFC, controlling for global fluctuations in the MR signal and the magnitude of task-dependent activity. Third, to assess the importance of functional coupling within this network for attention shifting, independent of stress effects, a multivariate linear regression analysis was performed with shift cost as the dependent variable and measures of prefrontal connectivity as predictors, controlling for stress (PSS scores) as a covariate. Finally, the influence of stress on functional connectivity was examined using a two-factor mixed-effects ANOVA and voxelwise t-tests of connectivity (R2) in stressed versus control subjects.

Reversibility Analysis.

To control for confounding variables unrelated to stress and assess the reversibility of stress effects on PFC function, 15 stress-exposed subjects were rescanned after one month of reduced stress, along with an equal number of control subjects to account for practice effects. A three-factor (stress, high vs. low; session, 1 vs. 2; and subject) mixed-effects ANOVA and post hoc t-tests (session 1, high vs. low stress; session 2, high vs. low stress) were used to evaluate the reversibility of stress effects on PSS scores, shift costs, and functional connectivity. Stress-by-session interactions confirmed the reversibility of connectivity effects within a search volume encompassing all areas showing a main effect of stress, averaged across both sessions. This approach ensured that the analysis region was independent of the interaction being tested. Since this search volume excluded several regions exhibiting smaller effects in session 1, simple t-tests were performed on the peak voxel within each cluster to investigate if these areas followed a similar trend.

Link to Article

Abstract

Relatively little is known about the long-term neurobiological sequelae of chronic stress, which predisposes susceptible patients to neuropsychiatric conditions affecting the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Animal models and human neuroimaging experiments provide complementary insights, yet efforts to integrate the two are often complicated by limitations inherent in drawing comparisons between unrelated studies with disparate designs. Translating from a rodent model of chronic stress where we have shown reversible disruption of PFC function, we show that psychosocial stress induces long-lasting but reversible impairments in behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures of PFC function in humans. Twenty healthy adults, exposed to 1 month of psychosocial stress, confirmed by a validated rating scale, were scanned while performing a PFC-dependent attention-shifting task. One month later, they returned for a second scanning session after a period of reduced stress, and their performance was compared with a twice-scanned, matched group of low-stress controls. Psychosocial stress selectively impaired attentional control and disrupted functional connectivity within a frontoparietal network that mediates attention shifts. These effects were reversible: after one month of reduced stress, the same subjects showed no significant differences from controls. These results highlight the plasticity of PFC networks in healthy human subjects and suggest one mechanism by which disrupted plasticity may contribute to cognitive impairments characteristic of stress-related neuropsychiatric conditions in susceptible individuals.

Introduction

We already know that chronic stress is bad for our mental health. It's linked to serious conditions like depression, anxiety, and schizophrenia, all of which affect a part of the brain called the prefrontal cortex (PFC). The PFC is like the brain's control center, responsible for things like focus, problem-solving, and working memory. While short-term stress can temporarily disrupt these functions, we don't fully understand how long-term stress impacts the PFC.

Brain imaging studies in humans suggest a connection, but they don't reveal the underlying mechanisms. To get a clearer picture, we can look at animal studies, specifically those using rats. These studies have shown that chronic stress physically changes the structure of the PFC in rats, damaging connections between brain cells. This damage impairs the rats' ability to shift their attention between tasks. Importantly, these changes are reversible once the stress is removed.

This study aimed to investigate if similar changes occur in humans experiencing chronic stress. We used a task that mimics the one used in rats to see if stress impacts attention shifting in humans similarly and if these changes are reversible.

Results

Chronic Stress Makes it Harder to Switch Focus

Participants were given a visual task that required them to shift their attention between different features of images, like color and movement. As expected, everyone found it slightly harder to shift their attention than to focus on a single feature repeatedly. However, individuals who reported higher levels of stress on a standardized questionnaire (Perceived Stress Scale, PSS) showed a significantly greater difficulty in shifting their attention. Notably, their overall speed and accuracy on the task were not affected, suggesting a specific impairment in attention shifting.

Stress Disrupts Communication Within the Brain's Control Center

Brain imaging (fMRI) revealed that attention shifting activates a network of brain regions, including the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), an area involved in higher-order thinking. The imaging data showed weaker connections between the DLPFC and other parts of the brain network in stressed individuals, particularly those involved in movement and spatial awareness. Importantly, the strength of these connections correlated with performance on the attention-shifting task. This suggests that the disruption in communication within this brain network might explain why stressed individuals struggle to shift their attention effectively.

Good News: The Brain Can Recover

To determine if stress directly causes these brain changes, we asked the stressed participants to return a month later, after their stressful period (exams) had passed. Their stress levels and attention-shifting abilities were reassessed and compared to a control group.

The good news? The previously stressed individuals showed no differences in stress levels or attention shifting compared to the control group after the month-long break. The brain imaging also showed that the connections within the DLPFC network had returned to normal.

Discussion

Our findings in humans mirror those observed in rats, indicating that chronic stress can disrupt the structure and function of the PFC, leading to attention-shifting difficulties.

This study has several important implications:

  • Stress Impacts the Human Brain: It provides further evidence that chronic stress doesn't just impact our mental well-being; it physically affects our brain's ability to function optimally.

  • Reversibility Offers Hope: The fact that these changes are reversible suggests that stress-reduction techniques and interventions could help to restore healthy PFC function.

  • Animal Models are Valuable: The study highlights the importance of animal models in understanding complex human conditions and developing effective treatments.

While promising, this study has limitations. The stressor used (exams) was specific, and the sample size was relatively small. Future research should investigate the effects of different types of stressors and include a more diverse population.

Overall, this study emphasizes the importance of managing chronic stress for maintaining a healthy brain and optimal cognitive function. It also highlights the incredible capacity of the human brain to adapt and recover from adversity.

Link to Article

Abstract

Relatively little is known about the long-term neurobiological sequelae of chronic stress, which predisposes susceptible patients to neuropsychiatric conditions affecting the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Animal models and human neuroimaging experiments provide complementary insights, yet efforts to integrate the two are often complicated by limitations inherent in drawing comparisons between unrelated studies with disparate designs. Translating from a rodent model of chronic stress where we have shown reversible disruption of PFC function, we show that psychosocial stress induces long-lasting but reversible impairments in behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures of PFC function in humans. Twenty healthy adults, exposed to 1 month of psychosocial stress, confirmed by a validated rating scale, were scanned while performing a PFC-dependent attention-shifting task. One month later, they returned for a second scanning session after a period of reduced stress, and their performance was compared with a twice-scanned, matched group of low-stress controls. Psychosocial stress selectively impaired attentional control and disrupted functional connectivity within a frontoparietal network that mediates attention shifts. These effects were reversible: after one month of reduced stress, the same subjects showed no significant differences from controls. These results highlight the plasticity of PFC networks in healthy human subjects and suggest one mechanism by which disrupted plasticity may contribute to cognitive impairments characteristic of stress-related neuropsychiatric conditions in susceptible individuals.

Chronic Stress Messes with the Brain

You already know that being stressed out isn't good for you. But did you know that long-term stress can actually change the way your brain works? It can even make it harder to think! Scientists have found that when people experience chronic stress – meaning they feel stressed for weeks or even months – it can have a negative impact on the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the part of your brain responsible for things like focusing, problem-solving, and controlling your impulses.

Scientists wanted to understand how stress affects the PFC. They already knew from studying rats that short bursts of stress mess with brain chemicals that help the PFC work. But what about long-term stress in humans?

To figure this out, the scientists asked a group of medical students to do a computer task that measures attention. They tested the students during a time when they were under a lot of stress: preparing for a big exam. They compared their performance to a group of similar people who weren't under the same stress.

The results? The stressed-out students had a much harder time with the attention task! Brain scans showed that their PFCs weren't communicating as well with other parts of the brain. It was like the connections were weaker.

But here's the good news: when the scientists tested the students again a month after their exams (when they were less stressed), their brains had bounced back! Their attention skills were back to normal, and the connections in their PFCs were strong again.

This study shows us a few important things:

  • Chronic stress can mess with your brain: It can make it harder to focus and pay attention, which are super important skills for school, work, and life in general.

  • Our brains are resilient: Even though stress can change our brains, these changes aren't necessarily permanent. When the stress goes away, our brains can often heal themselves.

  • Managing stress is crucial: Finding healthy ways to deal with stress – like exercise, meditation, or talking to someone you trust – can help protect your brain and keep it working at its best.

This research also helps scientists better understand how stress contributes to mental health conditions like anxiety and depression. The more we learn about the brain's response to stress, the better equipped we'll be to develop new and more effective treatments.

Link to Article

Abstract

Relatively little is known about the long-term neurobiological sequelae of chronic stress, which predisposes susceptible patients to neuropsychiatric conditions affecting the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Animal models and human neuroimaging experiments provide complementary insights, yet efforts to integrate the two are often complicated by limitations inherent in drawing comparisons between unrelated studies with disparate designs. Translating from a rodent model of chronic stress where we have shown reversible disruption of PFC function, we show that psychosocial stress induces long-lasting but reversible impairments in behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures of PFC function in humans. Twenty healthy adults, exposed to 1 month of psychosocial stress, confirmed by a validated rating scale, were scanned while performing a PFC-dependent attention-shifting task. One month later, they returned for a second scanning session after a period of reduced stress, and their performance was compared with a twice-scanned, matched group of low-stress controls. Psychosocial stress selectively impaired attentional control and disrupted functional connectivity within a frontoparietal network that mediates attention shifts. These effects were reversible: after one month of reduced stress, the same subjects showed no significant differences from controls. These results highlight the plasticity of PFC networks in healthy human subjects and suggest one mechanism by which disrupted plasticity may contribute to cognitive impairments characteristic of stress-related neuropsychiatric conditions in susceptible individuals.

How Stress Affects the Brain

Being Stressed Out Can Make It Harder to Focus

We all know that being stressed out is no fun. But did you know that stress can actually change how your brain works? Scientists have been studying how stress affects a part of the brain called the prefrontal cortex (PFC). This part of your brain helps you make decisions, focus your attention, and solve problems.

Scientists already knew that when people are stressed, their brains release special chemicals that make the PFC work differently for a short time. But they wanted to know how long-term stress affects the PFC. To study this, they used special brain scanners to look at the brains of medical students who were preparing for a big exam. They compared these brains to the brains of people who were not stressed.

The scientists found that the stressed students had a harder time switching their attention between different tasks. Their brains also showed different patterns of activity compared to the non-stressed people. This means that being stressed for a long time can actually change how different parts of your brain talk to each other!

The good news is that these changes seem to be reversible. When the students finished their exams and were no longer stressed, their brains went back to normal. This tells us that even though stress can have a powerful effect on the brain, our brains are also incredibly resilient and can bounce back.

Here are the main things that the scientists learned:

  1. Being stressed out for a long time can make it harder to focus and switch your attention.

  2. These problems might be caused by changes in how different parts of the brain communicate with each other.

  3. The good news is that these changes seem to be reversible once the stress goes away.

This study is important because it helps us understand how stress affects the brain. This knowledge can help us develop better ways to cope with stress and prevent its negative effects on our mental health.

Link to Article

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Liston, C., McEwen, B. S., & Casey, B. J. (2009). Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(3), 912-917. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0807041106

    Highlights