Impulsive and risky decision-making in adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): The Need for a Developmental Perspective
Tycho J. Dekkers
Erik de Water
Anouk Scheres
SummaryOriginal

Summary

Teens are prone to take risks and make impulsive choices. This is even more true for teens with ADHD. This study points out that we don't fully understand how this risk changes over time. The authors propose more research for clarity.

2022

Impulsive and risky decision-making in adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): The Need for a Developmental Perspective

Keywords Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; ADHD; Adolescence; impulsive decision making; risky decision making

Abstract

Impulsive and risky decision-making peaks in adolescence, and is consistently associated with the neurodevelopmental disorder Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), regardless of age. In this brief review, we demonstrate the similarity of theoretical models explaining impulsive and risky decision-making that originate in two relatively distinct literatures (i.e., on adolescence and on ADHD). We summarize research thus far and conclude that the presence of ADHD during adolescence further exacerbates the tendency that is already present in adolescents to make impulsive and risky decisions. We also conclude that much is still unknown about the developmental trajectories of individuals with ADHD with regard to impulsive and risky decision making, and we therefore provide several hypotheses that warrant further longitudinal research.

Introduction

Reckless driving, trying drugs, having unsafe sex, or conducting criminal behavior: impulsive or risky decision making peaks during the adolescent years, and is observed disproportionately often in individuals with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). This raises the questions (1) whether the presence of ADHD during adolescence further exacerbates the tendency that is already present in adolescents to make impulsive and risky decisions, and (2) whether going through adolescence further exacerbates the tendency that is already present in individuals with ADHD to make impulsive and risky decisions? The current review is centered on these two research questions.

While the real-life examples above are often interchangeably referred to as impulsive or risky acts, in psychological research, a distinction is made between impulsive and risky decision-making. Impulsive decision-making refers to a preference for small, immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards. Risky decision-making refers to a preference for choices with a high variability in possible outcomes (i.e., large uncertain rewards) over choices with lower variability in potential outcomes (i.e., small certain rewards).

According to dual-system models, impulsive and risky behaviors peak in adolescence as result of an imbalance between two brain systems and their interconnections: (a) The early maturation of the affective system (involving ventral striatum, amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex) that subserves emotion and reward processing, and (b) the slower maturation of the cognitive control system (involving lateral prefrontal cortex and lateral parietal cortex). Similarly, the dual-pathway model of ADHD proposes two pathways that rather independently lead to the expression of ADHD symptoms: (a) a dysregulated cognitive control pathway (involving the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and dorsal striatum) characterized by weak response inhibition and executive dysfunctioning, and (b) a reward pathway (involving ventral striatum, amygdala, ACC, and medial prefrontal cortex) characterized by a shortened delay reward gradient. While there is clear overlap in the systems proposed to play a role in adolescence and ADHD, there are also important differences. While an imbalance between systems is proposed to be key during adolescence, ADHD models do not necessarily suggest an imbalance, but rather propose that “deficits” in either of these systems can form a pathway to ADHD. Further, while the role of the cognitive control system is suggested to be comparable in adolescence and in ADHD (in both cases, a slowly developing/immature cognitive control system), this is not the case for the affective system. Dual-system theories suggest that adolescence-induced hyperresponsivity of this system leads to reward-seeking behaviors, whereas the dual-pathway model of ADHD proposes a unique motivational style related to ADHD, characterized by delay aversion.

Impulsive decision making

Impulsive decision making is typically assessed with temporal discounting (TD) tasks, which involve choices between a small, immediate reward (e.g., $2 today), and a larger, delayed reward (e.g., $10 after 30 days). Studies that included participants from a relatively narrow age range showed that late adolescents choose less impulsively than early adolescents on TD tasks, that adolescents make less impulsive decisions than children, but they make more impulsive decisions than young adults. However, studies including participants with a wider age range found that adolescents were less impulsive than both children and young adults, which could reflect the fact that adolescents may be able to engage the cognitive control system more than children and young adults when they are motivated by rewards. Achterberg et al. found linear decreases in impulsive decision making with age in cross-sectional analyses, but non-linear (i.e., decreased impulsive decision making in adolescents compared to young adults) effects of age in longitudinal analyses. This suggests that longitudinal designs may be more sensitive to detecting subtle individual differences and developmental trajectories.

Four recent meta-analyses examined group differences between ADHD and controls on impulsive decision making measured with TD and/or single choice tasks. Patros et al. reported impulsive decision making in children and adolescents with ADHD relative to typically developing controls. Group differences were large for preschoolers, and medium for school-age children and adolescents. Similarly, Jackson et al. reported increased impulsive decision making in individuals with ADHD relative to controls. Effect sizes were similar for children, adolescents and adults. Pauli–Pott et al. performed a meta-regression to evaluate ADHD-control differences on impulsive decision making as a function of developmental transition periods. Based on previous findings reporting a peak in impulsive choice during mid-adolescence, they hypothesized that group differences would be smallest during this period because these would be obscured by the inter-individual variance induced by puberty. Indeed, the effect size was larger for children than for adolescents. Marx et al. reported an up-to-date meta-analysis on ADHD-control comparisons (all ages). Between-group differences were small on single choice paradigms and medium on TD tasks. Effects were not moderated by age, but adolescents were not treated as a separate group.

This consistent increase in impulsive decision making in ADHD is considered a consequence of delay aversion. Sonuga–Barke proposed that individuals with ADHD experience negative emotions during waiting, causing a desire to escape this. Indeed, several studies support this notion in adolescents with ADHD. For example, adolescents with ADHD discounted delayed rewards, but not effortful rewards, more steeply than controls, suggesting a specific role for delay aversion. This was accompanied by a delay-sensitive response in the amygdala for those adolescents with ADHD who were more delay averse in daily life. Similarly, Van Dessel et al. found that adolescents with ADHD, relative to controls, demonstrated delay-related increases in the amygdala following cues signaling impending waiting times, which was interpreted as signaling aversion to waiting. In a behavioral study, impulsive decision making was more strongly correlated with the subjective experience of difficulty waiting in adolescents with ADHD relative to adolescents without ADHD. This suggests that adolescents with ADHD based their (impulsive) choice more on how difficult it feels to wait than controls. Similarly, Mies and colleagues [32] found that delay aversion—but not reward sensitivity—drove impulsive choices on TD tasks in adolescents with ADHD. Marx et al. showed that delay, independent of reward, exacerbated impulsive decision making in ADHD, also supporting the crucial role of delay aversion.

Taken together, in answer to question 1, we conclude that adolescents with ADHD engage in more impulsive decision making on experimental tasks than adolescents without ADHD (see Table 1, RQ1). As for question 2, there is no evidence suggesting that going through adolescence exacerbates the tendency that is already present in ADHD to make impulsive decisions (see Table 1, RQ2). If anything, there is some evidence that impulsive decision making is less characteristic of adolescents with ADHD than of children with ADHD. Delay aversion appears an important contributing mechanism of impulsive decision making in adolescents with ADHD.

Table 1

Risky decision making

Risky decisions are typically assessed with gambling tasks involving choices between low-risk options (e.g., high probability of small reward) and high-risk options (e.g., low probability of larger reward). A meta-analysis of experimental studies employing risky decision-making tasks showed that adolescents make more risky decisions than adults, but adolescents do not differ from children. Defoe et al. proposed that age differences in risk exposure (i.e., opportunities to engage in risky behaviors) might explain why adolescents show increased risk-taking compared to children in daily life, but not in experimental studies.

Adolescents with ADHD consistently engage in increased real-life risk-taking behavior relative to their typically developing peers. We discuss three meta-analyses summarizing all studies that compared groups with and without ADHD on gambling tasks, and we particularly focus on age-related differences.

The first meta-analysis found more risky decision making in groups with ADHD, with effects of small-to-medium size. Notably, the difference in risky choice between individuals with and without ADHD was similar across age. Second, an updated meta-analysis similarly observed a small-to-medium sized effect indicating increased risky decision-making in groups with ADHD. This meta-analysis distinguished between traditional gambling tasks and virtual reality (VR) measures of risky decision making, and demonstrated that group differences were larger when VR measures were used. For both types of outcome measures, age did not moderate the effects.

A third meta-analysis particularly focused on one underlying mechanism: suboptimal decision making. Groups with and without ADHD only differed in risky decision-making when risky decisions were suboptimal in terms of expected value, whereas there were no differences when risk taking was advantageous. This suggests that, at group level, individuals with ADHD rather show a tendency towards suboptimal decision making instead of an inherent risk proneness.

In conclusion, in answering question 1, ADHD is consistently associated with risky decision making, as indicated by three meta-analyses (Table 1). This effect was stable across age. There is no evidence for the notion that going through adolescence exacerbates the tendency to make risky decisions that is present in individuals with ADHD (question 2). Notably, the link between risky decision making and ADHD was limited to situations in which the risky choice was the suboptimal choice.

General discussion and future directions

We conclude that adolescents with ADHD show increased impulsive and risky decision making compared to typically developing adolescents (RQ1). Increased delay aversion in adolescents with ADHD may explain some of these group differences, particularly in impulsive decision making. There are many similarities between the neurobiological models of impulsive decision making developed for ADHD and for typical development, including the hypothesis that two interconnected brain systems play a role: an affective system and a cognitive control system. However, ADHD models emphasize that the affective system underpins both reward sensitivity and delay aversion while models of adolescence tend to focus on reward sensitivity alone. Nonetheless, recent research showed that in typically developing adolescents, impulsive decision making was associated with higher levels of delay aversion. Therefore, delay aversion may also be relevant for impulsive decision making in adolescents without ADHD.

The second research question, whether the tendency to make impulsive and risky decisions in individuals with ADHD is exacerbated during adolescence, is harder to answer. Few studies have used a developmental perspective in research on ADHD. While ADHD is regarded a neurodevelopmental disorder, the majority of research compares individuals with ADHD with age-matched controls, thereby missing pivotal knowledge about the development of individuals with ADHD. While a cross-sectional approach is informative to identify ADHD-control differences in decision making in different age groups (see Table 1, RQ2), longitudinal research is needed to examine ADHD-control differences in developmental trajectories of impulsive and risky decision making. Delineating such trajectories will enable the identification of key developmental periods during which interventions may be particularly helpful. Additionally, it will enable the identification of key developmental periods of impulsivity- and risk-related opportunities such as creativity, curiosity, and starting new social contacts. The following hypotheses, following from the dual system models, may be formulated (also see Figure 1).

  • (1) The developmental trajectory as observed in typical adolescents, namely a peak in impulsive and risky decision making in adolescence as compared to childhood and adulthood may be exaggerated in individuals with ADHD, for example, due to the delayed maturation of the prefrontal cortex in these individuals.

  • (2) As proposed by Pauli–Pott et al., the peak in impulsive and risky decision making in adolescence may be obscured in individuals with ADHD because puberty-related changes contribute to more variability in decision-making, thereby obscuring group differences.

  • (3) The developmental trajectories of adolescents with and without ADHD are similar in shape, but adolescents with ADHD are higher in impulsive and risky decision making than adolescents without ADHD.

Figure 1

Figure 1

When addressing such hypotheses, it will be important to consider the use of more ecologically valid tasks, including not only rewards but also losses and effort costs, not only monetary but also primary and social rewards, and integrating temporal and probabilistic aspects in relevant contexts. Additionally, while intuitively related, not many studies investigated the direct link between impulsive and risky choice in adolescents with ADHD, as most tasks only capture one of these constructs. An exception is the Cambridge Gambling Task (CGT), which disentangles risk proneness, risk adjustment (i.e., the adjustment to changes in the level of risk) and delay aversion. A few studies used the CGT in samples with ADHD, although not in adolescents. In children, groups with and without ADHD did not differ in their risk proneness, but children with ADHD had more problems adjusting their bets to changes in risk, and showed increased delay aversion. Crucially, these aspects were correlated, which suggests that risky decision making in relation to ADHD is potentially driven by a different motivational style in which immediate rewards are preferred to escape delay, which aligns with the motivational part of the dual pathway model.

Clinical implications

The research reviewed here may also contribute to the development of interventions to reduce impulsive and risky decision making in adolescents with ADHD. Delay aversion seems to play an important role in impulsive decision making, and may contribute to risky decision making as well. Commonly used psychotherapies such as mindfulness and acceptance and commitment therapy were found to decrease impulsive decision making and risky behaviors (e.g., smoking, overeating) in typically developing young adults. Further, short-term interventions such as episodic future thinking (e.g., vividly imagining a valued delayed reward during decision-making) appear to reduce impulsive decision making in typically developing young adults. Future studies should investigate the effectiveness of these interventions in adolescents with ADHD.

Contextual factors are crucial when developing such interventions, as affective brain systems are particularly activated in the presence and under the influence of peers. Empirically, this is mirrored by many studies demonstrating that typically developing adolescents engage in increased impulsive and risky decision making in the presence and under the influence of peers. In the sole experimental investigation of susceptibility to peer influence in ADHD, adolescents with and without ADHD were equally susceptible to peer influence on risky decision-making, whereas there are no experimental studies yet on susceptibility to peer influence with regard to impulsive choice in groups with ADHD.

A final direction for clinicians is to not only consider negative aspects of impulsive and risky decision-making (also see study by Green et al.). In clinical practice, adolescents with ADHD often report positive forms of impulsive or risky behaviors, such as asking others on a date, talking with strangers or trying new hobbies. Yet, empirical evidence for these observations is lacking, and reviews on studies in typically developing adolescents suggest the opposite: positive and negative risk taking were generally negatively correlated or entirely distinct, and adolescents with low self-regulatory capacities showed more negative and less positive risk taking. Also, self-reported impulsivity was linked to negative but not positive risk taking.

In conclusion, we found that adolescents with ADHD make more impulsive and risky choices than age-matched peers on lab-based tasks, with delay aversion playing a key role. An important question to be answered is which developmental trajectories in impulsive and risky decision making individuals with ADHD follow, as compared to controls. Longitudinal research is needed, using ecologically valid tasks. Clinically speaking, knowledge about these trajectories will give insight into time windows of opportunity and intervention.

Link to Article

Abstract

Impulsive and risky decision-making peaks in adolescence, and is consistently associated with the neurodevelopmental disorder Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), regardless of age. In this brief review, we demonstrate the similarity of theoretical models explaining impulsive and risky decision-making that originate in two relatively distinct literatures (i.e., on adolescence and on ADHD). We summarize research thus far and conclude that the presence of ADHD during adolescence further exacerbates the tendency that is already present in adolescents to make impulsive and risky decisions. We also conclude that much is still unknown about the developmental trajectories of individuals with ADHD with regard to impulsive and risky decision making, and we therefore provide several hypotheses that warrant further longitudinal research.

Impulsive and Risky Decision-Making in Adolescents with ADHD

Introduction

Adolescence is characterized by increased impulsive and risky decision-making. This tendency is disproportionately observed in individuals with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). This review examines two research questions:

  1. Does ADHD exacerbate the adolescent tendency towards impulsive and risky decision-making?

  2. Does adolescence exacerbate the tendency towards impulsive and risky decision-making in individuals with ADHD?

Impulsive Decision-Making

Impulsive decision-making involves prioritizing immediate rewards over delayed rewards. Studies consistently show that adolescents with ADHD exhibit increased impulsive decision-making compared to typically developing adolescents. This difference is attributed to delay aversion, a negative emotional experience during waiting that drives individuals with ADHD to seek immediate gratification.

Risky Decision-Making

Risky decision-making involves choosing options with high variability in potential outcomes. Meta-analyses indicate that individuals with ADHD engage in more risky decision-making than their peers, regardless of age. This tendency is linked to suboptimal decision-making, where individuals with ADHD prefer risky choices even when they are disadvantageous.

General Discussion and Future Directions

The neurobiological models of impulsive and risky decision-making in ADHD and typical adolescence share similarities, emphasizing the interplay between an affective system and a cognitive control system. However, ADHD models highlight the role of delay aversion in both reward sensitivity and impulsive decision-making.

Longitudinal research is needed to examine the developmental trajectories of impulsive and risky decision-making in individuals with ADHD. This will help identify key developmental periods for intervention and explore potential positive aspects of these behaviors.

Clinical Implications

Understanding the role of delay aversion in impulsive decision-making in ADHD can inform interventions such as mindfulness and episodic future thinking. It is also important to consider the influence of peers and explore the potential benefits of impulsive and risky behaviors in clinical practice.

Link to Article

Abstract

Impulsive and risky decision-making peaks in adolescence, and is consistently associated with the neurodevelopmental disorder Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), regardless of age. In this brief review, we demonstrate the similarity of theoretical models explaining impulsive and risky decision-making that originate in two relatively distinct literatures (i.e., on adolescence and on ADHD). We summarize research thus far and conclude that the presence of ADHD during adolescence further exacerbates the tendency that is already present in adolescents to make impulsive and risky decisions. We also conclude that much is still unknown about the developmental trajectories of individuals with ADHD with regard to impulsive and risky decision making, and we therefore provide several hypotheses that warrant further longitudinal research.

Impulsive and Risky Decision-Making in Adolescents with ADHD

Introduction

Adolescents are known for their tendency to make impulsive and risky decisions. This is even more pronounced in individuals with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). This article explores two questions:

  1. Do adolescents with ADHD make more impulsive and risky decisions than those without ADHD?

  2. Does going through adolescence make these tendencies worse in individuals with ADHD?

Impulsive Decision-Making

Impulsive decision-making means choosing immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards. Studies have shown that adolescents with ADHD are more likely to make impulsive choices than those without ADHD. This is thought to be due to delay aversion, where individuals with ADHD experience negative emotions while waiting and try to avoid it.

Risky Decision-Making

Risky decision-making means choosing options with uncertain outcomes over safer options. Studies have consistently shown that adolescents with ADHD engage in more risky behaviors than their peers. However, this difference may be limited to situations where the risky choice is not the best option.

Developmental Considerations

It is unclear whether adolescence worsens impulsive and risky decision-making in individuals with ADHD. Some studies suggest that these tendencies may be less pronounced in adolescents with ADHD compared to children with ADHD. However, more research is needed to determine the developmental trajectory of these behaviors in individuals with ADHD.

Clinical Implications

Understanding impulsive and risky decision-making in adolescents with ADHD can help develop interventions to reduce these behaviors. Interventions that focus on reducing delay aversion and improving decision-making skills may be beneficial.

Conclusion

Adolescents with ADHD tend to make more impulsive and risky decisions than their peers without ADHD. Delay aversion may play a key role in these differences. However, it is unclear how adolescence affects these tendencies. More research is needed to understand the developmental trajectory of impulsive and risky decision-making in individuals with ADHD and to develop effective interventions.

Link to Article

Abstract

Impulsive and risky decision-making peaks in adolescence, and is consistently associated with the neurodevelopmental disorder Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), regardless of age. In this brief review, we demonstrate the similarity of theoretical models explaining impulsive and risky decision-making that originate in two relatively distinct literatures (i.e., on adolescence and on ADHD). We summarize research thus far and conclude that the presence of ADHD during adolescence further exacerbates the tendency that is already present in adolescents to make impulsive and risky decisions. We also conclude that much is still unknown about the developmental trajectories of individuals with ADHD with regard to impulsive and risky decision making, and we therefore provide several hypotheses that warrant further longitudinal research.

Impulsive and Risky Decisions in Teens with ADHD

Teens with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) tend to make more impulsive and risky decisions than teens without ADHD. Impulsive decisions are those that a person makes when they choose something they want right away, even if it may not be the best choice in the long term.

Why Does This Happen?

There are a few reasons why teens with ADHD might make more impulsive and risky decisions:

  • Delay Aversion: Teens with ADHD often feel uncomfortable waiting for things they want. This can make them more likely to choose immediate rewards, even if they're smaller.

  • Brain Differences: The brains of teens with ADHD are wired a little differently. They have a harder time controlling their impulses and making good decisions.

  • Peer Influence: Teens with ADHD may be more likely to be influenced by their friends when making decisions.

Is It a Problem?

Impulsive and risky decisions can lead to problems like:

  • Trouble in school

  • Problems with relationships

  • Substance abuse

  • Accidents

The Good Side of Impulsivity and Risk-Taking

It's important to remember that not all impulsive and risky decisions are bad. Sometimes, taking a risk can lead to positive things like:

  • Trying new things

  • Making new friends

  • Being creative

Conclusion

Teens with ADHD face some challenges when it comes to making decisions. But with the right support, they can learn to make better choices and live successful lives.

Link to Article

Abstract

Impulsive and risky decision-making peaks in adolescence, and is consistently associated with the neurodevelopmental disorder Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), regardless of age. In this brief review, we demonstrate the similarity of theoretical models explaining impulsive and risky decision-making that originate in two relatively distinct literatures (i.e., on adolescence and on ADHD). We summarize research thus far and conclude that the presence of ADHD during adolescence further exacerbates the tendency that is already present in adolescents to make impulsive and risky decisions. We also conclude that much is still unknown about the developmental trajectories of individuals with ADHD with regard to impulsive and risky decision making, and we therefore provide several hypotheses that warrant further longitudinal research.

Teenagers with ADHD: Why They Take More Risks

Teenagers are known for taking chances, sometimes without thinking things through. This can be even more true for teenagers with ADHD (Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder). This review looked at two questions:

  • Do teenagers with ADHD take more risks than other teenagers?

  • Does being a teenager make ADHD worse in terms of risky behavior?

Making Quick Choices

Teenagers with ADHD often choose things that give them a reward right away, even if it means missing out on something better later. This is because they may dislike waiting and feel the need for instant gratification. Studies show this is more common in teens with ADHD than in other teens.

Taking Risks

People with ADHD are more likely to make choices that could have bad or unpredictable outcomes. For example, they might choose something risky even if it's not the best option. Research suggests this is because they might not fully consider all the possibilities before making a decision.

Why Teens with ADHD Might Be Different

Both normal teenagers and people with ADHD share some similar reasons for making impulsive and risky decisions. It has to do with how the brain balances emotions and thinking things through. However, for people with ADHD, disliking waiting seems to play a bigger role in both wanting rewards and making quick choices.

What We Need to Learn More About

Scientists need to follow teenagers with ADHD over time to see how their risk-taking behavior changes as they grow up. This can help identify the best ages to intervene and support them in making good decisions. It's also important to consider that some risky behavior might have positive aspects, and not all risk-taking is bad.

Helping Teens with ADHD Make Better Choices

Understanding why teenagers with ADHD dislike waiting can help develop better ways to help them make decisions. Techniques like mindfulness and thinking about the future might be useful. It's also important to consider the influence of friends and how risky behavior can sometimes be a positive way to explore and learn new things.

Link to Article

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Dekkers, T. J., de Water, E., & Scheres, A. (2022). Impulsive and risky decision-making in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): The need for a developmental perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 44, 330–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.11.002

    Highlights