Frontostriatal White Matter Integrity Predicts Development of Delay of Gratification: a Longitudinal Study
Michelle Achterberg
Jiska S Peper
Anna C K van Duijvenvoorde
René C W Mandl
Eveline A Crone
SimpleOriginal

Summary

Cognitive control for delayed gratification improves with age until late adolescence, then declines. Brain connections between decision-making and reward areas strengthen during childhood and early adulthood, influencing patience.

2016

Frontostriatal White Matter Integrity Predicts Development of Delay of Gratification: a Longitudinal Study

Keywords Teenagers; Brain development; White matter; Impulsivity; Self-control; adolescence; development; impulsivity; longitudinal

Abstract

The ability to delay gratification increases considerably across development. Here, we test the hypothesis that this impulse control capacity is driven by increased maturation of frontostriatal circuitry using a fiber-tracking approach combined with longitudinal imaging. In total, 192 healthy volunteers between 8 and 26 years underwent diffusion tensor imaging scanning and completed a delay-discounting task twice, separated by a 2-year interval. We investigated dynamic associations between frontostriatal white matter (WM) integrity and delay of gratification skills. Moreover, we examined the predictive value of frontostriatal WM integrity for future delay of gratification skills. Results showed that delay discounting increases with age in a quadratic fashion, with greatest patience during late adolescence. Data also indicated nonlinear development of frontostriatal WM, with relative fast development during childhood and early adulthood and--on average--little change during mid-adolescence. Furthermore, the positive association between age and delay discounting was further increased in individuals with higher WM integrity of the frontostriatal tracts. Predictive analysis showed that frontostriatal WM development explained unique variance in current and future delay of gratification skills. This study adds to a descriptive relation between WM integrity and delay of gratification by showing that maturation of frontostriatal connectivity predicts changes in delay of gratification skills. These findings have implications for studies examining deviances in impulse control by showing that the developmental path between striatum and prefrontal cortex may be an important predictor for when development goes astray.

Introduction

Between childhood and adulthood, vigorous advancements in the ability to sustain goal-directed cognition in the face of immediate rewards are observed (Eigsti et al., 2006; Olson et al., 2007; de Water et al., 2014). This ability to delay gratification can be captured in delay-discounting tasks, estimating an individual's preference for a smaller immediate reward over larger, delayed rewards. A crucial element of these tasks is that the subjective value of a reward decreases when the delay to that reward increases (Critchfield and Kollins, 2001). This capacity has been interpreted as an index of impulse regulation, which changes considerably during adolescence (van den Bos et al., 2015).

A leading hypothesis suggests that maturation of this type of impulse regulation capacity is driven by increased regulatory control of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) over reward-related striatal areas (Figner et al., 2010; Christakou et al., 2011; Casey, 2015). Several studies showed that the striatum is more activated by decisions involving immediately available rewards, whereas prefrontal and parietal cortices are activated when individuals control the temptation to choose immediate rewards (McClure et al., 2004; Somerville et al., 2011; Peters and Buchel, 2011). These results led to the question of whether maturation of prefrontal–striatal white matter (WM) connections concurs with and predicts future-oriented choices across development.

The integrity of connections between the striatum and prefrontal cortex can be assessed using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI). DTI measures the diffusion profile of water molecules in vivo, allowing us to probe microstructural properties of the connecting WM fiber bundles (Le Bihan and Breton, 1985; Jones, 2008). The measurements most commonly derived from DTI are fractional anisotropy (FA), measuring the directional variation of diffusion, and mean diffusivity (MD), measuring the amount of diffusion (Basser and Pierpaoli, 1996). Several DTI studies revealed higher WM integrity across adolescence (Olson et al., 2009; Bava et al., 2010; Simmonds et al., 2014; Peper et al., 2015), although the shape of the trajectory is not yet well understood, some reporting linear and others nonlinear changes (for an overview, see Schmithorst and Yuan, 2010). Moreover, recent studies in adults (Peper et al., 2013) and adolescents (Van den Bos et al., 2015) reported an association between higher frontostriatal WM integrity and increased preference for delayed rewards. From these studies, two important issues remain unresolved: (1) whether the relationship between age and discounting is eliminated—or merely diminished—when brain connectivity is taken into account (Steinberg and Chein, 2015) and (2) whether maturation of frontostriatal WM connections across development is an important predictor of individual development of delay of gratification skills.

To address these questions, the current study followed participants with ages ranging from childhood throughout early adulthood (age 8–26) over a 2-year period. This longitudinal design enabled us to test whether the association between age and discounting behavior is mediated by WM integrity between striatum and PFC and to move beyond a descriptive relation among age, WM integrity, and behavior by testing how brain maturation predicts change in behavior over time.

Consistent with the existing literature, we hypothesized that the ability to delay gratification improves with increasing age (Green et al., 1994; Olson et al., 2007; de Water et al., 2014) and that the integrity of frontostriatal WM matures with increasing age (Olson et al., 2009; Bava et al., 2010; Schmithorst and Yuan, 2010; Simmonds et al., 2014; Peper et al., 2015). The longitudinal design allowed us to test in more detail the shape of change (Braams et al., 2015). In addition, we hypothesized that the increasing effect of age on the ability to delay gratification is further increased in individuals with relatively high frontostriatal WM integrity (positive mediation) (Liston et al., 2006). Ultimately, we hypothesized that frontostriatal WM integrity predicts the improvement of delay gratification over time. That is to say, we expect that frontostriatal WM integrity at time point 1 can predict delay of gratification at time point 2 and that thereby brain maturation precedes and predicts behavioral change.

Materials and Methods

PARTICIPANTS.

The current study was part of a large longitudinal study, referred to as Braintime, conducted at Leiden University, the Netherlands. A total number of 299 participants (ages 8–25) were recruited through local schools and advertisements at time point 1 (T1). All participants were fluent in Dutch, right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and an absence of neurological or psychiatric impairments. Two years later, at time point 2 (T2), 254 participants were included. From the 254 participants who had measurements on both time points, 14 participants had missing delay discounting data at one of the two time points and 13 participants had missing DTI data at one of the two time points. Thirty-four participants were excluded due to erratic discounting behavior at one of the two time points. Consistent discounting behavior was defined as having at least two decreases in subjective value (indifference points) and not more than one increase in subjective value as time increased (Dixon et al., 2003). The excluded participants had similar demographic characteristics as the included participants (excluded participants: 50% male; age range 8.21–24.44; age at T2 M = 16.05, SD = 3.66). Behavioral and neuroimaging results with the excluded participants remained unchanged.

There were no outliers in delay discounting data (Z-value <−3.29 or >3.29). Outliers in DTI data were winsorized (Tabacknick and Fidell, 2013). The final longitudinal sample (participants included at T1 and T2) consisted of 192 participants (48.4% males; age range = 8.01–26.62; age at T2 M = 16.31, SD = 3.61; see Table 1 for demographic characteristics).

Table 1

Table 1

Demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 192, 48.4% male) at T1 and T2

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants or participant's parents in the case of minors. All anatomical MRI scans were reviewed and cleared by a radiologist from the radiology department of the Leiden University Medical Center (LUMC). No anomalous findings were reported. Participants received a financial reimbursement for their participation in a larger-scale study (Braams et al., 2014b, 2014a; Peters et al., 2014b, 2014a; van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2016). The institutional review board of the LUMC approved the study and its procedures.

Intelligence quotient (IQ) was estimated with the subsets “similarities” and “block design” at T1 and the subsets “vocabulary” and “picture completion” at T2 of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults, third edition (WAIS-III) or the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition (WISC-III). Different subsets were used to prevent learning effects. The demographic characteristics of the sample are listed in Table 1. There was no significant correlation between estimated IQ and delay of gratification skills at T1 (r = 0.0195, p = 0.195) nor at T2 (r = 0.113, p = 0.119). Therefore, IQ was not included as a covariate in the remaining analyses.

DELAY-DISCOUNTING TASK.

A computerized version of a hypothetical delay-discounting task described by Peper et al. (2013) was used, based on the paradigm explained by Richards et al. (1999). Subjects were asked to make a series of choices between either a small, immediately available amount of money or €10 available after a delay (i.e., “What would you rather have: €2 right away or €10 in 30 d?”). Discounting was assessed at 4 delays (2, 30, 180, and 365 d later). Trials with different delays were presented in a mixed fashion. Furthermore, the task was adaptive: after the choice for the immediately available money, this amount was decreased on a next trial, whereas if the delayed money was preferred, the amount of immediately available money on the next trial was increased (decreasing adjustment algorithm; Du et al., 2002).

The amount of immediately available money that the participant considered to be equivalent to the €10 delayed reward was taken to indicate the subjective value of the delayed rewards. Based on these so called “indifference points,” the area under the discounting curve (AUC) was obtained, an often-used measure of amount of discounting (Myerson et al., 2001). The normalized AUC ranges from 0 (complete discounting) to 1 (no discounting). The smaller the AUC, the faster people discount the delayed reward and the more impulsive (or delay aversive) they are. The task was presented as a hypothetical delay-discounting task. However, several studies have shown that choices on a hypothetical delay-discounting task substantially and significantly correlate (r's up to 0.74) with choices on a delay discounting task with real rewards in adults (Bickel et al., 2009; Scheres et al., 2010).

IMAGING ACQUISITION AND PROCESSING.

The same imaging acquisition was used as described in Peper et al. (2013). Scans were acquired on a 3 tesla Philips Achieva MRI system. Two transverse diffusion weighted imaging (DWI) scans were obtained with the following parameter settings: 30 diffusion-weighted volumes with different noncollinear diffusion directions with b-factor 1000 s/mm2 and 5 diffusion-unweighted volumes (b-factor 0 s/mm2); anterior–posterior phase encoding direction; parallel imaging SENSE factor = 3; flip angle = 90 degrees; 75 slices of 2 mm; no slice gap; reconstruction matrix 128 × 128; FOV = 240 × 240 mm; TE = 69 ms; TR = 7315 ms; total scan duration = 271 s per DWI set. The second DWI set had identical parameter settings as used for the first set except that it was acquired with a reversed k-space readout direction (posterior–anterior phase encoding direction), enabling the removal of susceptibility artifacts during postprocessing (Andersson et al., 2003). During scanning, the FOV was angulated according to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line and diffusion gradients were adjusted accordingly during data processing. Subsequently, diffusion scans were realigned to the averaged b0 scan and corrected for motion, eddy current, and susceptibility distortions (Andersson and Skare, 2002; Andersson et al., 2003). A tensor was fitted to the diffusion profile in each voxel using a robust tensor fitting method to correct for possible effects of cardiac pulsation and head motion (Chang et al., 2005; Chang et al., 2012). The main diffusion direction was determined as the principal eigenvector of the eigenvalue decomposition of this fitted tensor. Based on the eigenvalue decomposition, two measures derived from the diffusion tensor were computed: FA, which measures the directional variation of diffusion and ranges from 0 (no preferred diffusion direction) and 1 (highly preferred diffusion direction) and MD, measuring the amount of diffusion (Basser and Pierpaoli, 1996).

WM pathways were reconstructed using deterministic streamline tractography, based on the Fiber Assignment by Continuous Tracking (FACT) algorithm (Mori et al., 1999). Within each voxel of the cerebral WM, 8 streamlines were started, following the computed diffusion directions from voxel to voxel until one of the stopping criteria was reached (FA < 0.1, sharp turn of 45 degrees or more, or exceeding brain tissue). This procedure resulted in a collection of reconstructable WM tracts from which fiber tracts of interest could be selected.

FRONTOSTRIATAL VOLUME OF INTEREST.

We used a “volume of interest” (VOI) to measure frontostriatal WM tracts as described by Peper et al. (2013). The VOI requires that the fiber tracts that are reconstructed for each subject in native space are put into model space to create the VOI (for a detailed description, see Peper et al., 2013). In short, tracts were required to run through both the striatum and PFC to be included as frontostriatal WM. Inclusion regions of interest (ROIs) were based on the automatic anatomical labeling (AAL) template (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002), including the caudate, putamen, and pallidum (AAL regions 71–76), as well as the dorsolateral, ventrolateral, and ventromedial prefrontal cortices (AAL regions 5–10, 13–16, and 25–28). The ROIs were dilated with two voxels in all directions to ensure that they penetrate the WM. Exclusion ROIs were the genu of the corpus callosum (manually delineated on the midsagittal slice), the uncinate fasciculus, and the longitudinal fascicules (manually delineated by a plane through the temporal lobes where the amygdala was located). For fiber selection, all ROIs had to be defined only once on the model brain. For an individual example of frontostriatal fiber tracts, see Figure 1. All voxels within the selected frontostriatal tracts were flagged, resulting in individual binary maps of frontostriatal tracts (in model space) for each participant of the sample on both time points T1 and T2. Subsequently, the VOI was created for frontostriatal tracts of the sample: Every voxel within the frontostriatal tract should have a fiber running through in at least 50% of the sample (i.e., thresholded at 50%; Fig. 1). Then, this particular voxel was flagged and added to the VOI. The left and right hemisphere were combined to ensure comparability with earlier reports (Liston et al., 2006; de Zeeuw et al., 2012; Peper et al., 2013; van den Bos et al., 2015) that did not report hemispheric differences in relation to impulsive behavior.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The frontostriatal WM tract within an individual subject is displayed in yellow, with the striatum and PFC as inclusion ROIs. Red regions display the VOI. The VOI was created across the whole sample; a voxel was included when it had a frontostriatal fiber running through in at least 50% of the total sample.

Within the VOI of the frontostriatal tract, DTI metrics (FA and MD) were calculated for each individual subject of the whole sample.

GLOBAL WM

As a control measure of global WM development and to test for specificity of the contribution of frontostriatal WM tracts to delay discounting behavior, WM tracts of the whole brain—excluding the frontostriatal tracts—were examined as well.

STATISTICAL ANALYSES

Statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS version 21 and in R version 3.1.1 (Pinheiro et al., 2013). The contribution of sex and intelligence to delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized) were explored using independent-sample t tests and Pearson's correlation in SPSS. Pearson's correlation in SPSS was also used to investigate the stability of delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized) and WM integrity (FA and MD) over time. Furthermore, mediation analyses were performed to test whether the relation between age and delay discounting was mediated by frontostriatal WM integrity, measured by FA and MD. For correct comparison between FA and MD, we used z-values in the mediation analyses. The present study used a bootstrapping approach to mediation as implemented in the SPSS macros of Preacher and Hayes (2008). Confidence intervals (95% CI) were estimated using the bias-corrected bootstrap method (number of resamples = 10000) implemented in the macros.

Mixed models were used to investigate age-related change (linear, quadratic, or cubic) in delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized) and frontostriatal WM integrity (FA and MD). Analyses were performed with the nlme package in R (Pinheiro et al., 2013). Mixed models are particularly useful in longitudinal studies because these datasets have time points within participants and the mixed-model approach can recognize this type of data dependency. To test for developmental effects, we followed a formal model-fitting procedure (for a similar approach, see Braams et al., 2015). We started by using a null model that only included a fixed and a random intercept to allow for individual differences in starting points and to account for the repeated nature of the data. We fitted three polynomial age models with increasing complexity that tested the grand mean trajectory of age; that is, a linear, quadratic, and cubic age trend. Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1974) and Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978), both standardized model-fit metrics, were used to compare the different models. Lower AIC and BIC values indicates a better model fit. Log likelihood ratio tests were used between nested models to test which age trend best described the data. Reported p-values for the mixed models are based on log likelihood ratio tests. All models were fit with full information maximum likelihood estimates.

Ultimately, linear regression models in SPSS were used to test longitudinal prediction models. In specific, we tested whether frontostriatal WM integrity (FA and MD) at T1 could predict delay of gratification skills at T2 while taking into account delay of gratification performance at baseline.

Results

Age effects on delay discounting

Cross-sectional data showed that advanced age was related to a larger AUC (normalized), meaning less steep discounting of delayed rewards with age, at both T1 (r = 0.207, p = 0.004) and at T2 (r = 0.204, p = 0.004). Delay of gratification skills at T1 were positively correlated with delay of gratification skills at T2 (r = 0.543, p < 0.001).

The longitudinal analyses, testing for linear, quadratic, and cubic changes in delay discounting, showed that age-related change in delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized) was best described by a quadratic age-model (age1: β=.1.269, p < 0.001; age2: β = −0.568, p = 0.040; Table 2). This model indicates a “peak” in AUC during late adolescence/early adulthood (Fig. 2a). We also performed the analyses without the group of young adults (>18 years) to check for possible biases in model-fitting results due to a relative small sample size (n = 21). However, age-related change in delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized) was, conforming the analysis on the total sample, best described by a quadratic age model (age1: β=.1.274, p < 0.001; age2: β = −0.509, p = 0.033). Finally, with respect to behavioral performance, we tested potential sex differences. In the current dataset, there were no significant sex or sex × age interaction effects in delay of gratification.

Table 2

Table 2

AIC and BIC values for null, linear, quadratic, and cubic age models fitted separately for delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized), FA of the frontostriatal (FS) tract, and MD of the FS-tract.

Figure 2

Figure 2

Individual variability over time for AUC normalized (a), FA (b), and MD (c). Every line represents one individual, with AUC/FA/MD at T1 at the left side of the line and AUC/FA/MD at T2 at the right side of the line. The solid lines display the predicted value of the best-fitting age model. Dotted lines represent the 95% CI.

Age effects on the frontostriatal tract

Cross-sectional data at T1 and T2 showed that WM integrity of the frontostriatal tract increased with age. Age was significantly positively correlated with FA at T1 (r = 0.440, p < 0.001) and at T2 (r = 0.351, p < 0.001) and significantly negatively correlated with MD at T1 (r = −0.220, p = 0.002), but not at T2 (r = −0.089, p = 0.089). Moreover, WM integrity measures were positively correlated between T1 and T2 (FA: r = 0.611, p < 0.001; MD: r = 0.583, p < 0.001).

Longitudinal analyses revealed that age-related change in WM integrity (FA and MD) was best explained by a cubic age model (FA: age1: β = 0.152, p < 0.001; age2: β = −0.050, p = 0.006; age3: β = 0.047, p = 0.004; MD: age1: β = −0.00010, p < 0.001; age2: β = 0.00005, p = 0.018; age3: β = −0.00007, p = 0.001; Table 2). More specifically, our data indicate that FA mostly increased during childhood and early adulthood. The reversed pattern of FA changes was observed for MD (Fig. 2b,c). However, when the analyses performed on the sample of 8- to 18-year-old participants only, age-related change in WM integrity was best explained by a quadratic age-model (FA: age1: β = 0.137, p < 0.001; age2: β = −0.048, p = 0.005; age3: β = 0.022, p = 0.1220; MD: age1: β = −0.00011, p < 0.001; age2: β = 0.00005, p = 0.015; age3: β = −0.00003, p = 0.131). Additional analyses showed that there were no significant sex or sex × age interaction effects in WM integrity (nor in FA or in MD).

Mediation analyses

To investigate the relation between age and WM integrity in explaining variance in delay of gratification skills, we performed mediation analyses using the method of Preacher and Hayes (2008). At T1, the effect of age on delay of gratification (path c: β = 0.016, p = 0.004) was fully mediated by FA (path a: β=.123, p < 0.001; path b: β= 0.067, p = 0.0019; path c′: β = 0.008, p = 0.195; mediation effect a*b: 95% CI 0.0034–0.0140; p = 0.004; Fig. 3a). Furthermore, the effect of age on delay of gratification skills (path c: β = 0.016, p = 0.004) was significantly mediated by MD (path a: β = −0.0614, p = 0.002; path b: β = −0.059, p = 0.003; path c′: β = 0.012, p = 0.026; mediation effect a*b: 95% CI 0.0012–0.0076; p = 0.030).

Figure 3

Figure 3

Mediation models. The relation between age and delay of gratification skills is partly mediated by FA at T1 (a) and at T2 (b). Values are standardized regression coefficients and asterisks indicate significance coefficients (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001).

Partly overlapping results were found at T2: FA was a significant mediator of the association between age and delay of gratification skills (path c: β = 0.014, p = 0.005; path a: β = 0.097, p < 0.001; path b: β = 0.038, p = 0.047; path c′: β = 0.011, p = 0.046; mediation effect a*b: 95% CI 0.0004–0.0081; p = 0.061; Fig. 3b). However, MD within the frontostriatal-tract did not mediate the association between age and delay of gratification skills (path c: β = 0.014, p = 0.005; path a: β = −0.035, p = 0.083; path b: β = 0.012, p = 0.488; path c′: β = 0.015, p = 0.004; mediation effect a*b: 95% CI = −0.0028–0.0006; p = 0.517). Therefore, the relation between age and delay of gratification performance was mediated by WM integrity within the frontostriatal tract at both time points.

Longitudinal prediction

To test whether WM integrity of the frontostriatal tract could predict future discounting behavior, we performed a linear regression analysis with delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized), age, FA, and MD at T1 as predictors for delay of gratification skills at T2. The results showed that, in addition to delay of gratification skills at T1 (β = 0.504, p < 0.001), FA was a significant predictor (β = 0.158, p = 0.034) for delay of gratification skills at T2 (R2 total model = 0.321, R2 FA = 0.017; Fig. 4, Table 3). Age at T1 and MD did not significantly predict future discounting behavior. The same analyses were performed with nonlinear age changes (age2 and age3). On top of delay of gratification skills and FA at T1, age2 and age3 did not significantly predict future discounting behavior. Therefore, while accounting for behavioral performance at baseline, FA within the frontostriatal tract explains unique variance in future delay of gratification skills.

Figure 4

Figure 4

Delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized) at T2 was predicted by delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized) at T1 and FA of the frontostriatal tract at T1. The y-axis displays the unstandardized predictive value of the regression model with AUC (normalized), age, FA of the frontostriatal tract, and MD of the frontostriatal tract at T1 as predictors.

Table 3

Table 3

Linear regression predicting delay of gratification skills at T2 using delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized), age, FA, and MD at T1We also investigated whether delay of gratification skills at T1 was predictive of frontostriatal WM integrity at T2. We entered FA at T2 as dependent variables and FA, age, and delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized) at T1 as predictor. The same analyses were conducted with MD at T2 as a dependent variable. Linear regression analyses showed that both FA (β = −0.018, p = 0.763) and MD (β = −0.008, p = 0.895) at T2 were not significantly predicted by delay of gratification skills at T1.

Global WM effects

To test for the specificity of the frontostriatal tract in predicting discounting behavior, we performed a similar analysis with global FA and MD (i.e., all WM connections excluding the connections marked as frontostriatal tract). Longitudinal analyses revealed that age-related change in global WM integrity (FA and MD) was also best explained by a cubic age-model (FA: age1: β = 0.225, p < 0.001; age2: β = −0.095, p < 0.001; age3: β = 0.047, p = 0.004; MD: age1: β = −0.00010, p < 0.001; age2: β = 0.00005, p = 0.018; age3: β = −0.00007, p = 0.001). Age-related change between 8 and 18 years was—similar to the frontostriatal tracts—best explained by a quadratic age model (FA: age1: β = 0.227, p < 0.001; age2: β = −0.041, p < 0.001; age3: β = 0.017, p = 0.073; MD: age1: β = 0.00005, p = 0.063; age2: β = 0.00009, p < 0.001; age3: β = −0.00002, p = 0.123). Importantly, the linear regression analysis with delay of gratification skills (AUC normalized), age, FA, and MD at T1 as predictors for delay of gratification skills at T2 showed that global FA (β = 0.059, p = 0.539) and MD (β = 0.060, p = 0.802) did not predict future discounting behavior.

Discussion

Development in risk-taking tendencies and impulsive control have been attributed to an imbalance between subcortical and cortical brain regions (Somerville et al., 2010; Casey, 2015), but very few studies have examined the anatomical connections between these areas in relation to impulsive choice. One important dimension of impulsivity is the ability to delay gratification (Whelan et al., 2012). Next to examining developmental patterns in impulsive choice and frontostriatal WM integrity, the current study aimed to test whether the integrity of frontostriatal WM connections mediated and predicted the ability to delay gratification across development. We were able to demonstrate that age-related increases in the preference for delayed rewards (i.e., less impulsive choice) were significantly dependent on a better quality of connections between the PFC and striatum. Moreover, the longitudinal analysis revealed that stronger connectivity between striatum and PFC predicted less impulsive choices 2 years later.

The first question addressed in this study was to test age-related change in the ability to delay gratification between childhood and young adulthood. From our results it appears that delay of gratification is largest around late adolescence, followed by a slight decline in early adulthood. This finding fits well with a recent study on age-related changes in discounting of real rewards (Scheres et al., 2006). It appears that there is a gradual increase in delay of gratification skills between childhood and late adolescence, reaching a plateau in late adolescence/early adulthood. Prior studies also showed the most reward-oriented behavior in mid-adolescence and a steep increase in late adolescence in self-control (Steinberg et al., 2008; Olson et al., 2009; de Water et al., 2014). A possible explanation that follows from these findings is that adolescents, more so than children, flexibly apply self-control for the purpose of reward maximization, which levels off in early adulthood.

Next to developmental change, there was also evidence for consistency in behavior across sessions within individuals. That is, we found correlations between delay of gratification skills at T1 and T2, showing that participants who were better able to delay gratification at T1 were also better able to delay gratification at T2, which is consistent with prior studies (Audrain-McGovern et al., 2009; Anokhin et al., 2011). These results indicate a substantial level of trait-like, individual stability in delay of gratification skills in adolescence (Casey et al., 2011). These findings set the stage for examining the hypothesis in this study: how individual variation in behavior is mediated and predicted by striatum–prefrontal cortex connectivity.

In a set of longitudinal analyses, we investigated the age-related change in frontostriatal WM connections. Results indicated that the integrity of frontostriatal WM increases with age and seems to do so in a cubic fashion: the most pronounced increases in WM integrity appear to take place in preadolescence and young adulthood, with, on average, a relatively stable period in between. This finding corresponds to previous longitudinal studies demonstrating protracted maturation of large association fiber bundles (Bava et al., 2011; Lebel and Beaulieu, 2011). Studies testing for nonlinear relations in WM tracts are scarce (Olson et al. (2009) and reported similar cubic relations in WM, with the strongest changes in FA and MD during preadolescence and young adulthood. Importantly, in a longitudinal study, Simmonds et al. (2014) recently reported, in WM tracts connected to the PFC, a period of rapid growth in childhood, followed by a slowdown of growth in mid-adolescence and acceleration of growth again in late adolescence/early adulthood. Our longitudinal results also suggest a “plateau” in WM development during mid-adolescence, but this seems to be the result of larger variation in WM development during this period; some children show increases in WM integrity, whereas others remain stable or even show decreases. Our results add to increasing evidence obtained from several neuroimaging modalities showing large variability in brain activity, morphology, and connectivity during mid-adolescence (Scherf et al., 2012) suggesting a unique period of vulnerabilities and opportunities (Crone and Dahl, 2012). It should be noted, however, that, due to a relative smaller number of participants early adulthood (n = 21), the increase in WM integrity in this period must be interpreted with caution (Mills and Tamnes, 2014). Indeed, the analyses without these young adults hint toward highest values of WM integrity during late adolescence and leveling off thereafter. Therefore, replication of these results in a larger number of adults is warranted to typify the exact nature of frontostriatal WM development after adolescence.

Next, we tested whether WM integrity of frontostriatal connections was related to individual differences in the ability to delay gratification in adolescents, similar as to what has been reported previously in adults (Peper et al., 2013; van den Bos et al., 2014). Our results showed that WM integrity of the frontostriatal tract (specifically FA) mediated the relation between age and delay discounting, consistent with findings of a recent study on the relation between frontostriatal connectivity and adolescent delay discounting (van den Bos et al., 2015). However, it is not clear whether the relationship between age and delay discounting is eliminated or merely diminished when connectivity is taken into account (Steinberg and Chein, 2015). Our results on T1 show a full mediation (that the direct effect is no longer significant), whereas our results on T2 show a partly mediation.

Finally, we for the first time tested whether WM frontostriatal connectivity predicted change in delay discounting across development. Predicting change is important for potential early identification of adolescents who are prone to impulsive choice (see also Ullman et al., 2014). The results showed that frontostriatal WM integrity was a significant predictor of the ability to delay gratification 2 years later while taking into account delay of gratification performance at baseline. These findings indicate that brain structure is an important underlying mechanism for predicting change in behavior, whereas a reversed claim, behavior being a predictor for brain change, cannot be made based on the current study. Change in delay of gratification is partly driven by a more mature WM connectivity path between striatum and PFC.

Our current results are based on a hypothetical delay-discounting task. According to the economic literature, not actually paying the participant for the choices on the delay-discounting task could possibly undermine the participants' behavior as to how seriously they take the choices. Although our earlier reported results of hypothetical discounting in adults (Peper et al., 2013) were consistent with results of real discounting in adults (van den Bos et al., 2014) and several studies have shown that choices on hypothetical and real tasks significantly correlate in adults (Bickel et al., 2009; Scheres et al., 2010), it might be possible that adolescents are specifically influenced by the hypothetical aspect of our task. However, a recent study with a real-discounting task in a larger age range (8–25 years; van den Bos et al., 2015) revealed similar modulating relations between structural connectivity and delay discounting, suggesting that the use of a hypothetical task might not influence the findings significantly.

In conclusion, the current study provides crucial links for our understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying delay of gratification skills. The ability to delay gratification improves between childhood and early adulthood and this is predicted by the integrity of frontostriatal WM connections. This study adds to a descriptive relation between WM integrity and delay of gratification skills by showing that maturation of frontostriatal connectivity predicts improvements in delay of gratification skills over a 2-year period. These findings have implications for studies examining deviances in impulse control in adolescence, such as in cases of substance abuse or crime, by showing that the developmental path between striatum and PFC may be an important marker for when development goes astray.

Link to Article

Abstract

The ability to delay gratification increases considerably across development. Here, we test the hypothesis that this impulse control capacity is driven by increased maturation of frontostriatal circuitry using a fiber-tracking approach combined with longitudinal imaging. In total, 192 healthy volunteers between 8 and 26 years underwent diffusion tensor imaging scanning and completed a delay-discounting task twice, separated by a 2-year interval. We investigated dynamic associations between frontostriatal white matter (WM) integrity and delay of gratification skills. Moreover, we examined the predictive value of frontostriatal WM integrity for future delay of gratification skills. Results showed that delay discounting increases with age in a quadratic fashion, with greatest patience during late adolescence. Data also indicated nonlinear development of frontostriatal WM, with relative fast development during childhood and early adulthood and--on average--little change during mid-adolescence. Furthermore, the positive association between age and delay discounting was further increased in individuals with higher WM integrity of the frontostriatal tracts. Predictive analysis showed that frontostriatal WM development explained unique variance in current and future delay of gratification skills. This study adds to a descriptive relation between WM integrity and delay of gratification by showing that maturation of frontostriatal connectivity predicts changes in delay of gratification skills. These findings have implications for studies examining deviances in impulse control by showing that the developmental path between striatum and prefrontal cortex may be an important predictor for when development goes astray.

The Role of Frontostriatal White Matter Connections in Delay of Gratification Development

Introduction

The ability to delay gratification, or resist immediate rewards in favor of larger, delayed rewards, undergoes significant development from childhood to adulthood. This capacity is thought to be driven by increased regulatory control of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) over reward-related striatal areas. White matter (WM) connections between the striatum and PFC play a crucial role in this process.

Methods

This longitudinal study followed participants aged 8–26 over a 2-year period. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) was used to assess the integrity of frontostriatal WM connections. Participants also completed a delay-discounting task to measure their ability to delay gratification.

Results

Age-Related Changes in Delay of Gratification and WM Integrity

Delay of gratification skills improved with age, peaking in late adolescence/early adulthood. Similarly, frontostriatal WM integrity increased with age, with the most pronounced changes occurring in preadolescence and young adulthood.

Mediation Analysis

WM integrity within the frontostriatal tract mediated the relationship between age and delay of gratification. This suggests that age-related improvements in WM connectivity contribute to the enhanced ability to delay gratification.

Longitudinal Prediction

Importantly, frontostriatal WM integrity at baseline predicted improvements in delay of gratification 2 years later. This finding indicates that brain structure plays a significant role in predicting behavioral change over time.

Discussion

These findings provide evidence that the maturation of frontostriatal WM connections is crucial for the development of delay of gratification skills. This has implications for understanding deviations in impulse control in adolescence, such as substance abuse or criminal behavior. The developmental trajectory of these connections may serve as a marker for identifying individuals at risk for such behaviors.

Link to Article

Abstract

The ability to delay gratification increases considerably across development. Here, we test the hypothesis that this impulse control capacity is driven by increased maturation of frontostriatal circuitry using a fiber-tracking approach combined with longitudinal imaging. In total, 192 healthy volunteers between 8 and 26 years underwent diffusion tensor imaging scanning and completed a delay-discounting task twice, separated by a 2-year interval. We investigated dynamic associations between frontostriatal white matter (WM) integrity and delay of gratification skills. Moreover, we examined the predictive value of frontostriatal WM integrity for future delay of gratification skills. Results showed that delay discounting increases with age in a quadratic fashion, with greatest patience during late adolescence. Data also indicated nonlinear development of frontostriatal WM, with relative fast development during childhood and early adulthood and--on average--little change during mid-adolescence. Furthermore, the positive association between age and delay discounting was further increased in individuals with higher WM integrity of the frontostriatal tracts. Predictive analysis showed that frontostriatal WM development explained unique variance in current and future delay of gratification skills. This study adds to a descriptive relation between WM integrity and delay of gratification by showing that maturation of frontostriatal connectivity predicts changes in delay of gratification skills. These findings have implications for studies examining deviances in impulse control by showing that the developmental path between striatum and prefrontal cortex may be an important predictor for when development goes astray.

The Brain's Role in Delaying Gratification

How Our Brains Help Us Wait for What We Want

As we grow from childhood to adulthood, we get better at delaying gratification, or resisting the urge to choose immediate rewards over larger, delayed ones. This ability is important for making good decisions and achieving our goals.

Researchers have found that the development of this ability is linked to changes in the brain. Specifically, the connections between the striatum, a brain region involved in reward processing, and the prefrontal cortex, a region involved in decision-making, become stronger as we age.

The Study

In this study, researchers followed a group of participants from childhood to young adulthood, measuring their ability to delay gratification and the strength of their frontostriatal connections at two time points.

Results:

  • Delay of gratification skills improved from childhood to late adolescence, then leveled off in early adulthood.

  • The strength of frontostriatal connections also increased with age, with the most significant changes occurring in preadolescence and young adulthood.

  • The strength of frontostriatal connections was related to individual differences in the ability to delay gratification.

  • Importantly, the strength of frontostriatal connections at the first time point predicted improvements in delay of gratification skills 2 years later.

Conclusion

These findings suggest that the development of stronger connections between the striatum and prefrontal cortex plays a crucial role in our ability to delay gratification. This has implications for understanding why some adolescents may be more prone to impulsive behavior, as their frontostriatal connections may not be as well-developed.

Link to Article

Abstract

The ability to delay gratification increases considerably across development. Here, we test the hypothesis that this impulse control capacity is driven by increased maturation of frontostriatal circuitry using a fiber-tracking approach combined with longitudinal imaging. In total, 192 healthy volunteers between 8 and 26 years underwent diffusion tensor imaging scanning and completed a delay-discounting task twice, separated by a 2-year interval. We investigated dynamic associations between frontostriatal white matter (WM) integrity and delay of gratification skills. Moreover, we examined the predictive value of frontostriatal WM integrity for future delay of gratification skills. Results showed that delay discounting increases with age in a quadratic fashion, with greatest patience during late adolescence. Data also indicated nonlinear development of frontostriatal WM, with relative fast development during childhood and early adulthood and--on average--little change during mid-adolescence. Furthermore, the positive association between age and delay discounting was further increased in individuals with higher WM integrity of the frontostriatal tracts. Predictive analysis showed that frontostriatal WM development explained unique variance in current and future delay of gratification skills. This study adds to a descriptive relation between WM integrity and delay of gratification by showing that maturation of frontostriatal connectivity predicts changes in delay of gratification skills. These findings have implications for studies examining deviances in impulse control by showing that the developmental path between striatum and prefrontal cortex may be an important predictor for when development goes astray.

How Your Brain Helps You Wait for What You Want

What's the Deal with Delaying Gratification?

As you get older, you get better at waiting for things you want. This is called "delaying gratification."

How Does The Brain Help?

The brain has a special connection between two important areas: the striatum and the prefrontal cortex. The striatum is like the "go" button that makes you want things right away. The prefrontal cortex is like the "stop" button that helps you control your impulses and wait for what you want.

As people get older, the connection between these two areas gets stronger. This makes it easier for someone to resist the urge to act impulsively.

Why Is This Important?

Being able to delay gratification is a sign of maturity and self-control. It helps a person make better decisions and achieve their goals.

What the Study Found

Researchers studied a group of kids and young adults over two years. They found that:

  • Older kids and young adults were better at delaying gratification.

  • The stronger the connection between the striatum and prefrontal cortex, the better people were at delaying gratification.

  • The strength of this connection could predict how good someone would be at delaying gratification two years later.

What This Means

The teenage and young adult brain is still developing, and the connection between the striatum and prefrontal cortex is getting stronger. This means that people get better at delaying gratification as they get older. It also means that if a person practices controlling impulses now, they will be even better at it in the future.

Link to Article

Abstract

The ability to delay gratification increases considerably across development. Here, we test the hypothesis that this impulse control capacity is driven by increased maturation of frontostriatal circuitry using a fiber-tracking approach combined with longitudinal imaging. In total, 192 healthy volunteers between 8 and 26 years underwent diffusion tensor imaging scanning and completed a delay-discounting task twice, separated by a 2-year interval. We investigated dynamic associations between frontostriatal white matter (WM) integrity and delay of gratification skills. Moreover, we examined the predictive value of frontostriatal WM integrity for future delay of gratification skills. Results showed that delay discounting increases with age in a quadratic fashion, with greatest patience during late adolescence. Data also indicated nonlinear development of frontostriatal WM, with relative fast development during childhood and early adulthood and--on average--little change during mid-adolescence. Furthermore, the positive association between age and delay discounting was further increased in individuals with higher WM integrity of the frontostriatal tracts. Predictive analysis showed that frontostriatal WM development explained unique variance in current and future delay of gratification skills. This study adds to a descriptive relation between WM integrity and delay of gratification by showing that maturation of frontostriatal connectivity predicts changes in delay of gratification skills. These findings have implications for studies examining deviances in impulse control by showing that the developmental path between striatum and prefrontal cortex may be an important predictor for when development goes astray.

How Your Brain Helps You Wait for What You Want

What's the Deal with Waiting?

It is common to want something so strongly that waiting for it is difficult. But sometimes, we have to wait for the things we want. This is called "delaying gratification." As people grow up, they get better at delaying gratification. We learn that it's sometimes worth waiting for something we really want, even if it's hard.

Your Brain's Superhighway

Our brains have a special connection between two important parts: the striatum and the prefrontal cortex. It's like a superhighway that helps these parts talk to each other. This connection gets stronger as people get older.

The Superhighway and Waiting

Scientists have found that the stronger this superhighway is, the better we are at delaying gratification. It's like the superhighway helps our brain control our impulses and make good decisions.

Predicting the Future

Scientists have also discovered that the strength of this superhighway can predict how good we will be at delaying gratification in the future. So, if a person's superhighway is strong now, they are likely to be even better at waiting for what they want when they are older.

Why It Matters

Being able to delay gratification is important because it helps people make good choices and achieve their goals.

Link to Article

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Achterberg, M., Peper, J. S., van Duijvenvoorde, A. C., Mandl, R. C., & Crone, E. A. (2016). Frontostriatal White Matter Integrity Predicts Development of Delay of Gratification: A Longitudinal Study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(6), 1954–1961. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3459-15.2016

    Highlights