Early Health Risk Factors for Violence: Conceptualization, Evidence, and Implications
Jianghong Liu
SummaryOriginal

Summary

Bad health early in life, like poor nutrition for the mother or a difficult birth, may make a child more likely to be violent later. Focusing on a child's health from the very beginning may aid in preventing violence before it happens.

2011

Early Health Risk Factors for Violence: Conceptualization, Evidence, and Implications

Keywords Health Risk Factors; Alcohol during Pregnancy; Biosocial Interaction; Tobacco during Pregnancy; Toxicity and Drugs During Pregnancy; Head Injury During Early Childhood; Teenage Pregnancy; Birth Complications; Malnutrition of the Pregnant Mother; Malnutrition During Early Postnatal Years; Brain Dysfunction; Aggression; Child Behavior Problems; Violence Prevention; Protective Factors; Mediating Factors

Abstract

Violence and aggression are public health problems that can benefit from ongoing research into risk reduction and prevention. Current developmental theories of violence and aggression emphasize biological and psychosocial factors, particularly during adolescence. However, there has been less focus on understanding the interactive, multiplicative effects of these processes. Furthermore, little attention has been given to the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods, where prevention and intervention may yield effective results. Early health risk factors that influence negative behavioral outcomes include prenatal and postnatal nutrition, tobacco use during pregnancy, maternal depression, birth complications, traumatic brain injury, lead exposure, and child abuse. There is an ample literature to suggest that these early health risk factors may increase the likelihood of childhood externalizing behaviors, aggression, juvenile delinquency, adult criminal behavior, and/or violence. This paper proposes an early health risk factors framework for violence prediction, built on existing developmental theories of criminal behavior and supported by empirical findings. This framework addresses gaps in the adolescent psychopathology literature and presents a novel conceptualization of behavioral disturbance that emphasizes the pre-, peri-, and post-natal periods, when a child’s development is critical and the opportunity for behavioral and environmental modification is high. Implications for such a framework on violence prevention programs are discussed.

1. Introduction

Childhood aggression, teenage delinquency, and adult violent acts are being increasingly viewed as a public health issue. Identifying early risk factors is an important first step in preventing violence – a major societal problem. Despite decades of research into social and biological risk factors for antisocial and aggressive behavior in children and adults (Farrington et al., 2000; Virkkunen et al., 1995; Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1999), very little is known about effects of early childhood health factors on these outcomes. More specifically, research into the causes of antisocial and violent behavior has been hindered by three key empirical, conceptual, and methodological gaps in the literature.

1.1 Gaps in the Current Literature

The main empirical gap is that the role of early childhood health risk factors, such as prenatal factors, has been relatively neglected in the child and adolescent psychopathology literature. Recent research indicates that the prenatal stage of child development may be a key period in which effective interventions can potentially prevent later aggressive and violent behavior (Liu & Wuerker, 2005). Despite this, there remains a dearth of literature explicitly discussing the unique impact of prenatal contributors to these outcomes.

The second gap which is conceptual in nature is that to date theoretical frameworks of early risk factors for violence and aggression have not effectively integrated social and biological factors. Furthermore, health risk factors have been largely ignored from these frameworks. One exception is Raine et al. (1997), who more than a decade ago proposed a biosocial interaction model for understanding antisocial behavior. It is clear that childhood externalizing behaviors and adult violence are both byproducts of multiple processes. Violent behavior in adults and externalizing behavior in children are complex phenomena, and it is likely that neither biological nor psychosocial perspectives alone can adequately explain these outcomes. Considering early health factors along with biosocial processes provides a multi-perspective, cross-disciplinary approach that may improve the empirical foundation of the literature and lead to the development of more effective interventions.

The third gap which is methodological in nature concerns lack of prospective longitudinal studies on early health risk factors for negative behavioral outcomes. In the past, studies on childhood antisocial and aggressive behavior were mainly correlational and based on cross-sectional study designs or retrospective investigations. Prospective, longitudinal studies, while still correlational, can help tease out the temporal ordering of variables and assess whether the data are consistent with a causal model, particularly when structural equation modeling is used. For instance, a 15-year follow-up study on the impact of nurse home visits during prenatal and early childhood periods found that nurse-visited children displayed significantly fewer behavioral problems (e.g., arrests, running away, convictions) later in life than children who were not nurse-visited (Olds et al., 1998). Such studies provide compelling evidence that tackling early health risk factors may effectively reduce later aggressive and antisocial behavior and that longitudinal approaches can be used successfully to investigate these effects.

1.2 Current Theoretical Models and the Need for a New Approach

Some of the most important research on developmental theories on violence and aggression has emanated from the work of Moffitt, Farrington, and Fergusson. Moffitt’s dual pathway developmental theory of criminal behavior (Moffitt, 1993; Moffitt et al., 2002) categorizes antisocial adolescents according to their age of onset of deviant behavior (i.e., “early starters” versus “late starters”). “Early starters” are more likely to develop into life-course persistent (LCP) offenders rather than adolescent-limited offenders (as with “late starters”). Individuals in the LCP group are characterized by a combination of temperamental (e.g., negative emotionality) and neurological (e.g., inattention, hyperactivity, low IQ, low resting heart rate) antecedents that begin early in life and give rise to deviant behavior that steadily continues throughout adolescence, especially when combined with environmental deficits such as poor parenting. For instance, Moffitt et al., (2002) previously reported that childhood-onset delinquency had the strongest association with psychopathic personality traits, mental-health problems, substance dependence, greater number of children within the family, family financial problems, parental work problems, and drug-related and violent criminal behavior. Poor neurological status has also predicted male delinquency that began before age 13 years and persisted at high levels afterwards (Moffitt et al., 2006).

Farrington’s integrated cognitive antisocial potential theory (Farrington, 2005) parallels Moffitt’s framework in that it too emphasizes the predictability and developmental trajectories of criminal behavior, with particular attention paid to temperamental and neurological influences. His theory considers an individual’s antisocial potential (AP), or likelihood of engaging in antisocial behaviors, as well as cognitive thought processes that predispose to antisocial behavior. Farrington differentiates individuals with long-term AP from those with short-term AP, much like Moffitt’s LCP and adolescent-persistent distinction. Similar to Moffitt’s findings among LCP individuals, Farrington has reported that individuals with long-term AP are more likely to come from unstable family environments, exhibit lower IQ, display poor attention and hyperactivity, and have poor socialization skills. Indeed, Farrington noted that the most important childhood (ages 8-10 years) predictors of later delinquency were childhood antisocial behavior, impulsivity, low IQ and attainment, family criminality, poverty, large family size, and poor parental child-rearing behavior (Farrington 1995, 2000, 2001). What makes his approach particularly unique, however, is that it is based on data collected on multigenerational sources (Farrington & Walsh, 2007), allowing for a more thorough examination of both individual and family-related antecedents, such as poor parental supervision, parental conflict, and parental antisocial behaviors.

Support for these theories has come in part from important empirical testing by Fergusson and colleagues (Fergusson & Horwood 2001, 2002; Fergusson et al., 1994), who used longitudinal trajectory models to apply Moffitt’s and Farrington’s approaches and identify potential future offenders. Based on structural equation modeling, they observed that early childhood conduct problems predicted later delinquency and accounted for much of the relationship between IQ/academic achievement and delinquency (Fergusson and Horwood, 2001). These findings are consistent with those of Moffitt and Caspi (2001)who reported that childhood-onset delinquency – though not adolescent-onset delinquency – was associated with inadequate parenting, neurocognitive problems, and temperament and behavior problems during childhood. In a 21-year longitudinal study, Fergusson and Horwood (2002) identified male gender, parental criminality, parental conflict, novelty seeking, low IQ, and low self esteem as being most predictive of an offending trajectory.

While research from Moffitt, Farrington, and Fergusson has been influential in the field of developmental psychopathology, there is room for improvement. The children in Moffitt’s studies were all over 3 years of age. Similarly, Farrington’s samples were ages 10 years and older, and Fergusson’s samples were all ages 8 years and older. The theoretical model presented in this paper - the early health risk factor model - builds upon the work of these researchers by taking an integrated approach that includes health, biological, and psychosocial variables to better understanding predictors of negative behaviors, but it is unique in its application to early childhood. It is specifically designed to focus on the early childhood trajectory years and beyond) and developmental outcomes, and in this way can be conceptualized as an extension of Moffitt’s LCP model (Moffitt, 1993; Moffitt et al., 2002). Second, while Moffitt’s and Farrington’s models recognize the importance of biological as well as psychosocial factors, they do not necessarily emphasize the interactive effects between these variables. This is an important consideration, as possible synergistic effects may further increase the risk for negative outcomes beyond that of each of the risk factors independently. This framework addresses this oversight by proposing a greater focus on the interrelationships between a child’s biological and environmental background.

The field may benefit from a conceptual framework that both supports replicated findings about violence and aggression risk factors in children and adolescents while at the same time addressing the above described gaps, thus providing a relatively new perspective on understanding, predicting, and preventing these outcomes. This paper therefore introduces a new framework of early childhood health risk factors that stresses the multiplicative effects of biological, psychosocial, and environmental factors. The focus on early childhood health risk factors also helps inform the development of violence prevention programs, which may reduce childhood behaviors and possible prevent their escalation into more serious juvenile delinquency and adult crime. This framework is presented below, followed by a brief review of recent empirical finding to support this proposal.

2. An Early Health Conceptual Framework for Violence Study

2.1 Overview of the Conceptual Framework

The World Health Organization (WHO) defines health risk factors as behaviors and conditions that increase the likelihood for major injury, disease, or death (Ezzati et al., 2004). Among the 20 health risk factors that the WHO identifies as being of primary importance, preventable health risk factors include low childhood and maternal weight, tobacco and alcohol use, iron deficiency, obesity, high cholesterol and blood pressure, and unsafe sexual practices. These factors have considerable impact on longevity and quality of life, as worldwide estimates suggest they contribute to more than 29 million deaths annually (Ezzati et al., 2004).

This proposal for an early health risk factor framework (Figure 1) is built upon the author’s previous model of child externalizing behavior (Liu, 2004) which was derived from a biosocial model of violence (Raine et al., 1997). In this proposed new framework, health risk factors are defined as byproducts of biological and psychosocial processes during the pre-, peri-, and postnatal period and are hypothesized to be predictors of adverse outcomes, including childhood externalizing behavior, juvenile delinquency, and adult violence. Of note, the early health risk factors outlined in this framework focuses only on those which occur during prenatal period and childhood, are known to impair brain functions, and which would consequently be expected to influence behavior. Specifically, more negative behavioral conditions are hypothesized to be derived from prenatal insults or toxins, including fetal alcohol syndrome, and emerge on the basis of neural maldevelopment that results in cognitive dysfunction (Bishop & Rutter, 2009), in turn predisposing to life-course-persistent anti-social behavior (Moffitt, 1993). As a result, not all early health risk factors can be exhaustively encompassed since some are still lacking empirical evidence demonstrating their relationship to neurological deficits and violent/aggressive behaviors. In this framework, biological factors by themselves are hypothesized to directly give rise to behavioral outcomes. Similarly, social risk factors can independently and directly lead to externalizing behaviors. Importantly, the framework emphasizes that the interaction between the biological and psychosocial risk factors, which develops early health risk factors, along with potential mediators, may account for the relationship between the predictors and the expected outcomes.

Further discussion of the components of this proposed framework follows below, with subsequent descriptions of the empirical evidence supporting each component. Early childhood health risk factors assumed to increase the risk of childhood externalizing behavior and adult violence include exposure to alcohol, tobacco, toxicity and drugs during pregnancy; head injury during early childhood; teenage pregnancy; birth complications; and malnutrition both of the pregnant mother and the child in early postnatal years. This empirical review does not aim to cover all these health factors and will instead focus on those of greatest relevance to the proposed framework (summarized in Table 1).

2.2 Overview of Components of the Framework

2.2.1 Biological Risk Factors

During the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods, pertinent biological risk factors include both genetic and maternal pathophysiological processes that can impede fetal growth and development (Liu & Wuerker, 2005). While many early biological risk factors during the pre-and postnatal period may be linked to childhood externalizing behaviors, subsequent sections briefly review only a few of the most important factors, including maternal malnutrition, smoking during pregnancy, and birth complications.

2.2.2 Psychosocial Risk Factors

Psychosocial risk factors have been hypothesized to be associated with an increased risk for negative behavioral outcomes. Research on psychosocial risk factors has focused on child abuse (Widom, 1989), marital conflict (Margolin, 1988), poor parenting (Hodgins et al., 2001), poor child rearing (Webster-Stratton & Hammond 1999), domestic violence (Stoff et al., 1997), intimate partner violence (Campbell et al., 2007), and community violence (Scarpa & Haden, 2006). Other important considerations include low socioeconomic status, high psychosocial stress, negative attitudes about pregnancy, pregnancy during adolescence, and psychiatric disorders including substance abuse.

2.2.3 Protective Factors (Prevention and Intervention)

In this framework, protective factors are proposed to be introduced as early as before health risk factors are developed, and can serve to reduce biological vulnerability and intervene psychosocial influence each individually as well as to break down the interaction effects which produce early health risk factors. Protective factors increase one’s resilience, enhance resistance to risk, and strengthen individual defenses against the development of negative outcomes. The prenatal developmental period is intuitively an important time in which to focus on preventative measures because the developing fetus is highly vulnerable to influences from the environment such as maternal nutrition, toxins, and other health-related variables. Thus, proper prenatal care, including regular physical examinations, proper nutrition, general healthy lifestyle, avoidance of toxic chemicals (e.g., tobacco and substance use), as well as strong parental bonding and family harmony, are vital for risk reduction and increasing the chance of a positive outcome despite risk exposure.

2.2.4 Health Risk Factors: The Interaction between Biology and the Psychosocial Environment

Etiology of child externalizing behavior, delinquency, and adult violence is complex, and it is likely that both genes and environment contribute (Baker et al., 2008). An interaction refers to the effects of two risk factors are not merely additive, but are instead multiplicative. The framework argues that the likelihood of early health factors leading to later externalizing behavior would be strongly increased when biological risk factors combine with social risk factors. For example, birth complications, an early health risk factor which has been repeated found to predispose children to later violence (Liu et al., 2009) can be due to both biological risk factors (e.g. poor nutrition) and psychosocial risk factors (e.g. lack of prenatal care). The argument that psychosocial and biological factors interact to influence behavior is also support by other studies. Maternal cigarette smoking has been found to interact with paternal cigarette smoking to increase the risk of juvenile offending (Gibson & Tibbetts, 2000). Rasanen et al. (1999) found that maternal smoking combined with social risk factors (teenage pregnancy, single family status, and unwanted pregnancy), increased the odds ratios for violent offending by up to 9-fold for non-persistent offenses and up to 14-fold for persistent offenses. Furthermore, Caspi et al. (2002) also found that maltreated children with a genotype conferring high levels of monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A) expression were less likely to develop antisocial problems. Kim-Cohen et al. (2006) then further reported that mental health problems are significantly stronger in males with the genotype conferring low MAOA activity.

Recently, epigenetics - the study of inherited alterations in gene expression brought about by mechanisms other than changes in the underlying DNA sequence – has gained increasing attention in health risk factor research as recognition of multiple influences on maternal-fetal pathways has emerged. Epigenetic factors appear important in predicting behavioral outcomes as they may impinge on the neurodevelopment of the fetus’ growing brain. For example, exposure to environmental chemicals during the prenatal period may affect gene transcription, which subsequently influences the development of epigenetic disorders (Fushiki, 2009). Given that biology and the social environment are intertwined and may even have synergistic effects as opposed to independent effects, they are collectively defined unitarily as “health risk factors”.

This framework also recognizes the reciprocal interaction between biological and psychosocial risk factors. The reciprocal interaction is thus defined because psychosocial factors can give rise to biological risk factors, just as biological risk factors can predispose certain individuals to social risk factors. For example, teenage pregnancy as one component of social adversity is generally viewed as a psychosocial risk factor, but it could be that genetic/biological traits (e.g. high hormone levels) may predispose some teenager to engage in sexual activities and become pregnant. Another example of reciprocal interaction is that substance abuse is often viewed as a social-behavioral problem (Curran et al., 2000), but it could also be argued that individuals who abuse drugs and alcohol have a genetic/biological vulnerability to such behavior (Raine et al., 1997).

Thus, this framework proposes that early health risk factors, which results from the interaction between biological and psychosocial risk factors, are critically important in predisposition of later aggression, hyperactivity, and delinquency.

2.2.5 The Mediating Factor of the Brain

Related to the concept of interactive effects, resulting in early health risk factors, is mediating effects, which help explain the mechanism of action when an outcome is observed. Low IQ is among the most-replicated cognitive mediating factors for behavior problems (Hirachi & Hindelang, 1977; Moffitt, 1990). As discussed previously, findings from Liu et al. (2009) document that low IQ mediates the relationship between birth complications and externalizing behaviors. Other authors have similarly found that low IQ mediates the relationship between prenatal biological factors and externalizing outcomes, including such factors as nutrition (Liu, 2004; Liu et al., 2005), maternal tobacco exposure (Weitzman et al., 2002), and maternal alcohol use (Korkman et al., 2003). Furthermore, longitudinal studies have suggested that neuropsychological deficits characterize child and adult violence and antisocial behavior (Raine, 2002), and early neurocognitive deficits precede the onset of these antisocial behaviors (Moffitt, 1990).

Spatial IQ is defined as the non-verbal ability to perform complex visuo-spatial tasks (such as putting blocks together to form a pre-specified pattern, work one’s way through a maze) and is related to problem solving abilities in mathematics, engineering, architecture, science, art, and games. Low spatial IQ at age 3, as well as low verbal and spatial IQ at age 11, predicted Life-Course-Persistent antisocial behavior in community children (Raine et al., 2002). Similar results, showing that impairments in spatial abilities, but not verbal abilities, have been identified among antisocial children (Spelz et al., 1999). A possible mechanism by which low IQ in general functions as a mediating factor could be through its numerous psychosocial influences, including decreased bonding with parents, school failure, occupational failure, and low self-esteem – all of which increase the likelihood for antisocial behavior (Liu et al., 2010).

Regarding specific components of intelligence, verbal IQ on the widely-used Wechsler scales has been more strongly correlated with achievement than performance IQ (Kaufman & Lichtenberger, 2006). The predictive relationship between low verbal intelligence and later criminal behavior may reflect low academic achievement and school difficulties in general.

The relationship between spatial abilities and antisocial behavior may be due to right hemispheric damage, which would result in visuospatial deficits. Right hemisphere dominance aids in facial recognition and orientation, attention, and arousal in infants and young children. Impairment to this brain hemisphere could hinder child-parent bonding and affective processing and regulation (Raine et al., 2005). As such, in this proposed framework cognition is hypothesized to mediate the biosocial risk on externalizing and violent behaviors.

2.2.6 Impact of Early Health Risk Factors on Neurodevelopment and Subsequent Behaviors

Raine (2002) and Raine et al. (2002) present evidence from brain imaging, neuropsychological, and neurological studies that uniformly suggest damage or dysfunction to the prefrontal cortex as a significant predisposition to antisocial behavior. Low IQ and neurocognitive deficits manifested in the right hemisphere, and specifically in the visual and motor cortices, appear particularly important. These deficits can impair a child’s ability to establish visual and physical connections with the parents, thereby reducing parental bonding. Individuals who show significant instrumental aggression have shown evidence of amygdala dysfunction, resulting in resistance to aversive conditioning and passive avoidance learning (Blair, 2004).

The processes that can induce neurocognitive deficits are numerous. Sowell et al. (2007)collected functional MRI data showing that children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure exhibit functional abnormalities in the left medial temporal and dorsal prefrontal lobes and difficulties, areas which are pertinent in verbal learning. De Haan et al. (2006) as well as others (Sie et al., 2000) have put forth imaging evidence that perinatal asphyxia in term birth infants can lead to damage to the brainstem, thalamus, basal ganglia, parasagittal areas, and hippocampus. In an earlier study by Robertson et al. (1989), among infants who survived peri-natal asphyxia 25% showed significant neurological impairments, including encephalopathy, which has been correlated with impaired general intellectual abilities, slower academic progress, and poorer visual-motor integration in school-aged children. Non-accidental head injury, including shaken-baby syndrome, can cause diffuse neurological insult particularly to the prefrontal cortex. It is no surprise therefore that these injuries are significantly associated with a range of negative neurological outcomes, including a greater incidence of convulsions and subsequent hypoxia; sensory processing and motor deficits; language difficulties; and memory, attention, and other executive dysfunction (Ashton, 2010). Of note, infants and very young children appear to be especially prone to these negative outcomes (Ashton, 2010). Bellinger et al (2008) reports that lead exposure may preferentially inhibit functioning in several areas of the brain, including the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, postcentral gyri, inferior parietal lobule, and cerebellum. More recently, brain imaging studies show that exposure to lead during childhood is associated with decreased gray matter volume in adulthood, especially in portions of the prefrontal cortex which are responsible for mood regulation and decision-making (Cecil et al., 2008), which in turn predisposes these individuals to violent and criminal behavior. Consequently, lead exposure has been associated with outcomes related to poor impulse control, including violent offending, substance use, and ADHD (Bellinger et al., 2008).

3. Empirical Evidence of Health Risk Factors during Prenatal and Early Childhood

The following section briefly reviews the current empirical evidence supporting components of this framework. This includes the following health risk factors: prenatal maternal smoking; malnutrition; lead exposure; birth complications; childhood head injury; maternal depression and stress; and child abuse. While these do not exhaustively represent all health risk factors of importance to behavioural outcomes, they reflect factors that are most salient, are well-supported by the literature and, to a large degree are amenable to prevention and intervention strategies.

3.1 Smoking during Pregnancy

There is increasing evidence recognizing a significant association between smoking during pregnancy and later conduct disorder and violent offending (Ashford et al., 2008; D’Onofrio et al., 2010; Gibson & Tibbetts, 2000; Raine, 2002; Rasanen et al., 1999, Wakschlag et al., 2009), even after controlling for demographic, parental, and perinatal risk confounds (Brennan et al, 1999, 2002,). In a large birth cohort of males (N = 4169), Brennan et al. (1999) found a two-fold increase in adult violent offending among offspring of mothers who smoked 20 or more cigarettes per day during pregnancy, with an increasing dose-response relationship between number of cigarettes smoked and rates of violence. Furthermore, a recent study has shown that even second-hand exposure to cigarette smoking in the mother has been associated with increased externalizing psychopathology in the child (Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, 2007). The exact mechanism through which maternal tobacco exposure predisposes to behavioral problems in offspring is unclear. However, carbon monoxide and nicotine have been implicated in numerous areas of declining neurofunctioning, including noradrenergic disruption and serotonin and dopamine regulation (Muneoka et al., 1997); reductions in brain glucose (Eckstein et al., 1997); and basal ganglia, cerebral cortex, and cerebellar cortex dysfunction (Olds, 1997; Raine, 2002).

3.2 Malnutrition

Nutrition factors play an active and critical role in brain development during pregnancy and early childhood. Human studies suggest that a deficiency in macronutrients (e.g. protein), micronutrients (e.g. the trace elements zinc and iron), and a component of omega-3 fatty acids (docosahexaenoic acid or DHA – a long-chain essential fatty acid) can disturb brain functioning and thus further predispose to behavior disorders in children (Breakey, 1997; Fishbein, 2001; Lister et al., 2005; Liu & Raine, 2006; Werbach, 1992). Neugebauer et al., (1999) found that the male offspring of mothers severely malnourished during the first and second trimesters of pregnancy had 2.5 times the normal rate of antisocial personality disorder in adulthood. Postnatally, prospective findings indicate that children with protein, zinc, iron, and vitamin B deficiencies at age 3 years exhibited greater antisocial, aggressive, and/or hyperactive behaviors at ages 8, 11, and 17 years and the effects are mediated by cognitive deficits (Liu et al., 2004; Liu et al., 2003). Zinc deficiency has been specifically associated with greater violence and aggression (Liu et al., 2004; Watts, 1990) as well as hyperactivity (Brophy, 1986). In animal models, monkeys and rats deprived of the amino acid tryptophan show higher rates of aggression (Bjork et al., 1999), possibly due to the role of tryptophan in synthesizing serotonin, which has been shown to be reduced in individuals with impulsivity and violent behaviors (Virkkunmen et al., 1995; Werbach, 1995). Low iron has also been linked to aggression, conduct disorder, and juvenile delinquency (Rosen et al., 1985; Werbach, 1995). Furthermore, iron supplementation was shown improve cognitive and behavioral outcomes among non-iron deficient children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Sever et al., 1997).

The precise mechanism of action in these relationships is not well understood. There is speculation that proteins or minerals play a role in either regulating neurotransmitters or hormones, or else exacerbate neurotoxins, and in doing so predispose to brain dysfunction, aggression, and hyperactivity (Coccaro et al., 1997; Ferris & Grisso, 1996; Liu & Raine, 2006). Despite these promising findings, the research literature on malnutrition and externalizing behaviors remains both limited and controversial (Liu et al., 2004).

3.3 Lead Exposure

The toxic effects of lead on neurodevelopment are well-known, along with their consequent impact on behavior and development (Chen & Rogan, 2005; Dietrich et al., 2004; Koller et al., 2004; Needleman et al., 1996; Bellinger et al., 2005). Both animal studies (Delville, 1999) and human studies have found that higher lead levels measured either in the bone or blood are significantly related to aggressive and delinquent behavior in childhood (Burns et al., 1999; Needleman et al., 1996) as well as in early adulthood (Wright et al., 2008). Monkeys exposed to lead show patterns of behavioral impairment resembling the deficits of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Rice, 2000). Burns et al. (1999) found that school children from a lead-smelting community had increased externalizing behavior problems. This effect remained after controlling for a number of confounding variables, including quality of the child’s home environment, maternal psychopathology, and the child’s IQ. Sciarillo et al. (1992) found that children with elevated blood lead had behavior problems scores that were 2.7 times those of the controls. In addition, even children with low lead exposure may suffer from impaired cognitive performance (Koller et al., 2004). For example, Needleman (1996)found that children with elevated bone lead levels but no symptoms of lead toxicity showed higher aggression and delinquency scores compared to low-lead counterparts (Needleman, 1996). More recently, Wright et al. (2008) found that elevated blood lead concentrations during the prenatal and postnatal are associated with higher rates of criminal arrests in early adulthood. Nevertheless, studies in this area are to date few and limited, with many studies limited by small sample size or restriction to males only.

It is recognized that elevated blood lead concentrations are more common among children living in poverty, likely accounting for the mediating role of socioeconomic status between lead and behavioral outcomes (ATSDR, 1992; Hubbs-Tait et al., 2005). Environmental toxicity, micronutrients deficiency, and negative social environments may all produce adverse cognitive and behavioral outcomes (Moore, 2005), underscoring the importance of recognizing combined “health risk factors” (i.e. early health, biological, and psychosocial factors).

3.4 Birth Complications

Birth complications have been associated with antisocial behavior, particularly in the context of mitigating factors such as socioeconomic status and family stress. A large sample (N=4,269) of Danish males demonstrated an association between birth complications and violent behavior in adulthood when combined with early maternal rejection of the child (Raine et al., 1994). This has been replicated in four other international samples (Sweden, Finland, Canada, and United States). Prospective longitudinal research has similarly linked obstetric complications in the context of disadvantaged familial environment to increased adult violent crime (Piquero & Tibbetts, 1999). The interaction between pregnancy complications and poor parenting also predicts to adult violence (Hodgins, Kratzer, & Mcneil, 2001), as does the interaction between increased serious obstetric complications and family adversity (Arsenault et al., 2002). For instance, in a sample of Finnish males (N=5,587), the interaction between perinatal risk and being an only child raised the odds of adult violent offending by a factor of 4.4 (Kemppainen et al., 2001).

Recent evidence suggests that cognition might represent a possible third variable accounting for the relationship between birth complications and later externalizing behavior. Liu et al. (2009) examined the influence of birth complications in predisposing to externalizing behaviors at age 11 years. Birth complications included prenatal factors such as hypertension, low blood albumin, bleeding, and other illness (e.g., diabetes, pneumonia, asthma, and epilepsy). Perinatal factors consisted of breech fetal positioning, premature rupture of membrane, fetal distress, and use of instrument delivery (e.g., forceps, Cesarean section). Postnatal factors included cyanosis and treatment with oxygen. Among a sample of nearly 1,800 children, low IQ was significantly associated with both birth complications and externalizing behavior and was found to mediate the birth complications-externalizing behavior relationship. Birth complications may directly contribute to brain dysfunction by impeding proper neurodevelopment. For example, hypoxia selectively damages the hippocampal formation, thereby impairing the limbic system, a brain area repeatedly documented by brain imaging research to be associated with aggression regulation and disinhibition (Liu & Wuerker, 2005; Raine, 2002).

3.5 Head Injury

The literature linking even minor head injuries to violent behavior has consistently implicated traumatic brain injury in children in increasing emotional dysregulation and behavioral problems, including externalizing behaviors (Hatzitaskos et al., 1994; Andrews et al. 1998; Hatzitaskos et al., 1994; Max et al., 1998). Findings from neuropsychological assessments indicate significantly greater frontal and temporal dysfunction among antisocial individuals as compared to controls (Moffitt, 1990). Neuropsychological impairment may in part be a function of poor maternal prenatal conditions (e.g., alcohol and tobacco exposure). Furthermore, brain imaging studies have found that aggressive prisoners exhibit significantly reduced glucose metabolism in the prefrontal cortex relative to matched controls (Raine et al., 1997a). While findings are variable, antisocial and violent individuals as a group appear to be more likely to demonstrate neurocognitive deficits relative to non-violent controls.

3.6 Maternal Depression

Maternal depression has been consistently found to increase the potential for negative behaviors in the offspring (Hoffman et al., 2006; Phelan et al., 2007). In one sample, maternal depression was associated with a six-fold increase in the risk for childhood externalizing behavior (Mohan et al., 1998). More recently, a large-scale longitudinal study found that maternal depression was significantly associated with externalizing behavior boys at the age of 24 months, but by the time the children entered first grade, the relationship was stronger amongst girls (Blatt-Eisengart et al., 2009). This is consistent with other researchers who have noted a moderating effect of gender on the relationship between maternal depression and externalizing behavior (Burt et al., 2005; Wall and Holden, 1994). The contribution of maternal mental health on childhood aggression may be mediated by family factors, such as intra-family conflict and degree of spousal support (Malik et al., 2007) or other maternal characteristics, such as hostile and controlling behaviors (Marchand et al., 2002).

3.7 Maternal Stress

Although not outlined in the proposed framework, maternal stress should also be recognized as an important, overarching component of health risk factors as it can result from any of the above risk factors. Prenatal maternal stress is viewed as exposure of a pregnant woman to distress - either psychosocial or environmental - which induces biochemical changes in the mother that subsequently adversely affect the fetus’ growing organ systems, including the brain. As a multifaceted factor, prenatal stress has been studied in various forms, such as chronic anxiety (Sjostrom et al., 1997) and psychosocial stress (Entringer et al., 2009). Associations have been found between prenatal stress and low birth weight (Wadhwa et al., 2001). Additionally, findings from animal studies have linked prenatal stress to neurodevelopmental modifications in rats (Glover et al., 2009). However, while recent research has demonstrated an association between prenatal stress and the developing fetus (particularly the development of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal, or HPA, axis, - Glover et al., 2009) the link between prenatal stress and behavior remains largely unresolved.

Prenatal stress can be measured through biomarkers, such as cortisol level measurements, or through self-report, such as the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Maternal anxiety as measured by the STAI was thought to be related to a down regulation in placental 11β-HSD2, an enzyme that protects the fetus from high levels of circulating maternal cortisol (Glover et al., 2009). High cortisol levels are associated with adverse birth outcomes and may alter fetal development and subsequent adult health (Evans et al., 2008). Furthermore, prenatal stress can be denoted retrospectively. Study shows that experiencing a major negative life event was found to be associated with an alteration in the fetus’ hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning (Entringer, et al., 2009). Such changes are associated with physical and mental disorders, and the study authors concluded that psychosocial stress has a strong impact on an offspring’s mental and behavioral development. Prenatal stress, including exposure to toxicants such as cocaine during pregnancy, has been shown to be linked to elevated salivary cortisol stress reactivity in school-aged children (Lester et al., 2010), and Susman et al. (2010) found that high cortisol reactivity in early adolescent children was directly related to antisocial and rule- breaking behavior.

3.8 Child Abuse

It is not surprising that child abuse and neglect are strongly associated with a host of negative outcomes, including behavioral disturbance in adolescence and later adulthood (Widom, 1989; Teicher, 2000). Abused and neglected children were nearly five times as likely to be arrested for juvenile delinquency, with Caucasian youth demonstrating a staggering 20-fold increase in risk for juvenile arrest for violent crime (English et al., 2002). Moreover, the odds of arrest for criminal behavior as an adult were twice as high amongst victims of abuse. These trends were demonstrated in both males and females. Child maltreatment has also been linked to other negative outcomes, such as drug use and psychiatric illness (Kelly et al., 1997; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2008), which may indirectly increase the odds for aggression and violent behavior. In a review of the long-term consequences of exposure to abuse, Teicher (2000) identified four possible pathways that may explain such findings, including abnormalities in limbic functioning (predisposing to inhibition failure and poor emotional regulation), left hemispheric damage to the hippocampus (resulting in memory impairment), decreased activation of cross-hemispheric connections, and dysregulation of emotion and attention resulting from impairment to the vernis of the cerebellum. From a psychosocial perspective, low self-esteem – a common feature of abuse victims (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services) – has been found to significantly increase the odds of aggression over time (Donnellan et al., 2005).

4. Significance of the Framework and Implications

While the proposed framework should be viewed as heuristic, it may nevertheless provide an improved conceptual framework to better understand and prevent violent and externalizing behavior in children and adolescents. Research is needed in order to test the hypotheses suggested by this framework. Nonetheless, the significance and potential contribution made by the framework can be viewed at four levels.

First, violence is a problem that exacts significant social, psychological, and economic tolls. Childhood externalizing behavior is a well-documented predisposition to adult violence (Farrington, 1989, 2001; Ferris et al., 1996; Surgeon General, 2001). Increased awareness of contributing factors to childhood externalizing behavior can therefore help better elucidate the etiology of violence and crime. Second, the development of interventions to reduce aggression in children hinges on the ability to identify early risk factors that are amenable to change. For instance, Webster-Stratton and Hammon (1997)found that parental training that results in improved competence and maternal parenting skills was associated with a reduction in childhood conduct problems. Consequently, research on risk factors may help inform the development of such prevention programs. Additionally, because violence is considered a public health problem, prevention research plays a vital role in informing public policy and acquiring the economic support needed by local, state, and national agencies to implement violence prevention programs. From a theoretical perspective, the early health risk factor framework emphasizes interactive effects between biological and psychosocial processes, which allows for the examination of health risk factors from the main effect perspective as well as the interactive or biosocial perspective. Thus, it may help provide a clearer theoretical framework for future research on violence prediction and prevention.

Lastly, from a clinical perspective, there has been little recognition of the ways in which general medicine can contribute to violence prevention, but the framework’s emphasis on the early childhood developmental period may help facilitate greater discussion about the role of public health prevention initiatives, including those among general medicine. Health promotion programs implemented early in the child’s life, such as pre- and post-natal home visits by nurses, have been shown to play an important role in reducing future negative behaviors, including juvenile delinquency (Olds et al., 1998). Olds and colleagues (1998) found that children of mothers who received nurse home visits during the pre- and postnatal period displayed strikingly fewer antisocial behaviors at age 15 years than children of mothers who did not receive such visits, including 55% fewer arrests and 80% fewer criminal convictions. Nurse-visited children also had significantly lower rates of cigarette and alcohol use (Olds et al., 1998). Health visitation and wellness programs may be particularly useful for at-risk individuals, such as single and low-income mothers.

While there may certainly be a role for general medicine in helping to shape future child behavior, it should be noted that there are individual differences in susceptibility to early risk factors. For instance, given the same level of lead exposure in the same environment, some children do not develop high blood lead levels or behavioral problems, while others clearly do. These individual differences might reflect effects of protective or ameliorative factors, such as nutrition and parenting. More studies need to be conducted to understand why some risk factors do not uniformly result in adverse outcomes. Along these lines, one shortcoming of this proposed framework is that while interactions between the factors described earlier likely result in violent and aggressive behavior, there is a lack of empirical data to explain the underlying mechanisms of action in these interactions. Further research is needed before definitive conclusions can be made about cause-and-effect relationships and the underlying mechanisms.

Use of public prevention programs may be particularly well-suited to the early childhood developmental period. Tremblay’s research on the prevention of criminal behavior, in which he characterizes the preschool years as being “the best window of opportunity” for successfully intervening to reduce physical aggression (Tremblay, 2002), suggests that optimal results come with three important caveats. First, the intervention should be applied early on in the child’s life rather than waiting until adolescence. Second, it should addresses multiple risk factors rather than a single factor. Third, it should be conducted over an extended period of time (Tremblay & Craig, 1995). Specifically, targeting parenting behaviors, socially disruptive behaviors, and cognitive skills is advocated. Such an intervention might include teaching children verbal skills to mediate their use of physical aggression, helping young children learn to delay gratification, and teaching children cooperative skills to improve social interactions (Tremblay & Craig, 1995). School- and home-based programs can effectively apply such interventions or work closely with parents to impart these skills to children early on, before aggressive behavior escalates throughout the developmental trajectory.

The question of “nature versus nurture” has long been debated and continues to be controversial even today. In the past, sociologists have placed a great emphasis on social factors, while biological psychologists argued that violent behavior is rooted in pathophysiology. By focusing on the early developmental period, this framework serves as an extension of Moffitt’s LCP pathway, and the emphasis of this framework on interaction effects is consistent with the multifactorial approach (i.e., developmental, psychosocial, and environmental) described by Moffitt, Farrington, Raine, Fergusson, and others. This framework’s conceptualization is easily adaptable to prevention efforts, such as those suggested by Tremblay, as early health risk factors are amenable to intervention, as indicated by findings for Olds’ longitudinal study. By concentrating on early, modifiable risk factors, health professionals can play a greater role in secondary and tertiary prevention. This symbolizes a new and exciting interface between the contributions of scientists, educators, and practitioners alike, and may help address the longstanding public health concern of how to prevent violence.

Link to Article

Abstract

Violence and aggression are public health problems that can benefit from ongoing research into risk reduction and prevention. Current developmental theories of violence and aggression emphasize biological and psychosocial factors, particularly during adolescence. However, there has been less focus on understanding the interactive, multiplicative effects of these processes. Furthermore, little attention has been given to the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods, where prevention and intervention may yield effective results. Early health risk factors that influence negative behavioral outcomes include prenatal and postnatal nutrition, tobacco use during pregnancy, maternal depression, birth complications, traumatic brain injury, lead exposure, and child abuse. There is an ample literature to suggest that these early health risk factors may increase the likelihood of childhood externalizing behaviors, aggression, juvenile delinquency, adult criminal behavior, and/or violence. This paper proposes an early health risk factors framework for violence prediction, built on existing developmental theories of criminal behavior and supported by empirical findings. This framework addresses gaps in the adolescent psychopathology literature and presents a novel conceptualization of behavioral disturbance that emphasizes the pre-, peri-, and post-natal periods, when a child’s development is critical and the opportunity for behavioral and environmental modification is high. Implications for such a framework on violence prevention programs are discussed.

Summary

The prevalence of childhood aggression, teenage delinquency, and adult violence has led to its categorization as a public health crisis. Early identification of risk factors is crucial for preventing violence, a significant societal concern. While research has explored social and biological factors contributing to antisocial and aggressive behavior, a limited understanding exists about the impact of early childhood health factors on these outcomes. This paper identifies three key gaps in the literature: the underrepresentation of early childhood health risk factors, the lack of integrated theoretical frameworks encompassing social and biological factors, and the shortage of longitudinal studies examining early health risk factors.

1. Introduction

Childhood aggression, teenage delinquency, and adult violent acts are being increasingly viewed as a public health issue. Identifying early risk factors is an important first step in preventing violence – a major societal problem. Despite decades of research into social and biological risk factors for antisocial and aggressive behavior in children and adults (Farrington et al., 2000; Virkkunen et al., 1995; Webster-Stratton & Hammond, 1999), very little is known about effects of early childhood health factors on these outcomes. More specifically, research into the causes of antisocial and violent behavior has been hindered by three key empirical, conceptual, and methodological gaps in the literature.

1.1 Gaps in the Current Literature

The main empirical gap is that the role of early childhood health risk factors, such as prenatal factors, has been relatively neglected in the child and adolescent psychopathology literature. Recent research indicates that the prenatal stage of child development may be a key period in which effective interventions can potentially prevent later aggressive and violent behavior (Liu & Wuerker, 2005). Despite this, there remains a dearth of literature explicitly discussing the unique impact of prenatal contributors to these outcomes.

The second gap which is conceptual in nature is that to date theoretical frameworks of early risk factors for violence and aggression have not effectively integrated social and biological factors. Furthermore, health risk factors have been largely ignored from these frameworks. One exception is Raine et al. (1997), who more than a decade ago proposed a biosocial interaction model for understanding antisocial behavior. It is clear that childhood externalizing behaviors and adult violence are both byproducts of multiple processes. Violent behavior in adults and externalizing behavior in children are complex phenomena, and it is likely that neither biological nor psychosocial perspectives alone can adequately explain these outcomes. Considering early health factors along with biosocial processes provides a multi-perspective, cross-disciplinary approach that may improve the empirical foundation of the literature and lead to the development of more effective interventions.

The third gap which is methodological in nature concerns lack of prospective longitudinal studies on early health risk factors for negative behavioral outcomes. In the past, studies on childhood antisocial and aggressive behavior were mainly correlational and based on cross-sectional study designs or retrospective investigations. Prospective, longitudinal studies, while still correlational, can help tease out the temporal ordering of variables and assess whether the data are consistent with a causal model, particularly when structural equation modeling is used. For instance, a 15-year follow-up study on the impact of nurse home visits during prenatal and early childhood periods found that nurse-visited children displayed significantly fewer behavioral problems (e.g., arrests, running away, convictions) later in life than children who were not nurse-visited (Olds et al., 1998). Such studies provide compelling evidence that tackling early health risk factors may effectively reduce later aggressive and antisocial behavior and that longitudinal approaches can be used successfully to investigate these effects.

1.2 Current Theoretical Models and the Need for a New Approach

Some of the most important research on developmental theories on violence and aggression has emanated from the work of Moffitt, Farrington, and Fergusson. Moffitt’s dual pathway developmental theory of criminal behavior (Moffitt, 1993; Moffitt et al., 2002) categorizes antisocial adolescents according to their age of onset of deviant behavior (i.e., “early starters” versus “late starters”). “Early starters” are more likely to develop into life-course persistent (LCP) offenders rather than adolescent-limited offenders (as with “late starters”). Individuals in the LCP group are characterized by a combination of temperamental (e.g., negative emotionality) and neurological (e.g., inattention, hyperactivity, low IQ, low resting heart rate) antecedents that begin early in life and give rise to deviant behavior that steadily continues throughout adolescence, especially when combined with environmental deficits such as poor parenting. For instance, Moffitt et al., (2002) previously reported that childhood-onset delinquency had the strongest association with psychopathic personality traits, mental-health problems, substance dependence, greater number of children within the family, family financial problems, parental work problems, and drug-related and violent criminal behavior. Poor neurological status has also predicted male delinquency that began before age 13 years and persisted at high levels afterwards (Moffitt et al., 2006).

Farrington’s integrated cognitive antisocial potential theory (Farrington, 2005) parallels Moffitt’s framework in that it too emphasizes the predictability and developmental trajectories of criminal behavior, with particular attention paid to temperamental and neurological influences. His theory considers an individual’s antisocial potential (AP), or likelihood of engaging in antisocial behaviors, as well as cognitive thought processes that predispose to antisocial behavior. Farrington differentiates individuals with long-term AP from those with short-term AP, much like Moffitt’s LCP and adolescent-persistent distinction. Similar to Moffitt’s findings among LCP individuals, Farrington has reported that individuals with long-term AP are more likely to come from unstable family environments, exhibit lower IQ, display poor attention and hyperactivity, and have poor socialization skills. Indeed, Farrington noted that the most important childhood (ages 8-10 years) predictors of later delinquency were childhood antisocial behavior, impulsivity, low IQ and attainment, family criminality, poverty, large family size, and poor parental child-rearing behavior (Farrington 1995, 2000, 2001). What makes his approach particularly unique, however, is that it is based on data collected on multigenerational sources (Farrington & Walsh, 2007), allowing for a more thorough examination of both individual and family-related antecedents, such as poor parental supervision, parental conflict, and parental antisocial behaviors.

Support for these theories has come in part from important empirical testing by Fergusson and colleagues (Fergusson & Horwood 2001, 2002; Fergusson et al., 1994), who used longitudinal trajectory models to apply Moffitt’s and Farrington’s approaches and identify potential future offenders. Based on structural equation modeling, they observed that early childhood conduct problems predicted later delinquency and accounted for much of the relationship between IQ/academic achievement and delinquency (Fergusson and Horwood, 2001). These findings are consistent with those of Moffitt and Caspi (2001)who reported that childhood-onset delinquency – though not adolescent-onset delinquency – was associated with inadequate parenting, neurocognitive problems, and temperament and behavior problems during childhood. In a 21-year longitudinal study, Fergusson and Horwood (2002) identified male gender, parental criminality, parental conflict, novelty seeking, low IQ, and low self esteem as being most predictive of an offending trajectory.

While research from Moffitt, Farrington, and Fergusson has been influential in the field of developmental psychopathology, there is room for improvement. The children in Moffitt’s studies were all over 3 years of age. Similarly, Farrington’s samples were ages 10 years and older, and Fergusson’s samples were all ages 8 years and older. The theoretical model presented in this paper - the early health risk factor model - builds upon the work of these researchers by taking an integrated approach that includes health, biological, and psychosocial variables to better understanding predictors of negative behaviors, but it is unique in its application to early childhood. It is specifically designed to focus on the early childhood trajectory years and beyond) and developmental outcomes, and in this way can be conceptualized as an extension of Moffitt’s LCP model (Moffitt, 1993; Moffitt et al., 2002). Second, while Moffitt’s and Farrington’s models recognize the importance of biological as well as psychosocial factors, they do not necessarily emphasize the interactive effects between these variables. This is an important consideration, as possible synergistic effects may further increase the risk for negative outcomes beyond that of each of the risk factors independently. This framework addresses this oversight by proposing a greater focus on the interrelationships between a child’s biological and environmental background.

The field may benefit from a conceptual framework that both supports replicated findings about violence and aggression risk factors in children and adolescents while at the same time addressing the above described gaps, thus providing a relatively new perspective on understanding, predicting, and preventing these outcomes. This paper therefore introduces a new framework of early childhood health risk factors that stresses the multiplicative effects of biological, psychosocial, and environmental factors. The focus on early childhood health risk factors also helps inform the development of violence prevention programs, which may reduce childhood behaviors and possible prevent their escalation into more serious juvenile delinquency and adult crime. This framework is presented below, followed by a brief review of recent empirical finding to support this proposal.

2. An Early Health Conceptual Framework for Violence Study

2.1 Overview of the Conceptual Framework

The World Health Organization (WHO) defines health risk factors as behaviors and conditions that increase the likelihood for major injury, disease, or death (Ezzati et al., 2004). Among the 20 health risk factors that the WHO identifies as being of primary importance, preventable health risk factors include low childhood and maternal weight, tobacco and alcohol use, iron deficiency, obesity, high cholesterol and blood pressure, and unsafe sexual practices. These factors have considerable impact on longevity and quality of life, as worldwide estimates suggest they contribute to more than 29 million deaths annually (Ezzati et al., 2004).

This proposal for an early health risk factor framework (Figure 1) is built upon the author’s previous model of child externalizing behavior (Liu, 2004) which was derived from a biosocial model of violence (Raine et al., 1997). In this proposed new framework, health risk factors are defined as byproducts of biological and psychosocial processes during the pre-, peri-, and postnatal period and are hypothesized to be predictors of adverse outcomes, including childhood externalizing behavior, juvenile delinquency, and adult violence. Of note, the early health risk factors outlined in this framework focuses only on those which occur during prenatal period and childhood, are known to impair brain functions, and which would consequently be expected to influence behavior. Specifically, more negative behavioral conditions are hypothesized to be derived from prenatal insults or toxins, including fetal alcohol syndrome, and emerge on the basis of neural maldevelopment that results in cognitive dysfunction (Bishop & Rutter, 2009), in turn predisposing to life-course-persistent anti-social behavior (Moffitt, 1993). As a result, not all early health risk factors can be exhaustively encompassed since some are still lacking empirical evidence demonstrating their relationship to neurological deficits and violent/aggressive behaviors. In this framework, biological factors by themselves are hypothesized to directly give rise to behavioral outcomes. Similarly, social risk factors can independently and directly lead to externalizing behaviors. Importantly, the framework emphasizes that the interaction between the biological and psychosocial risk factors, which develops early health risk factors, along with potential mediators, may account for the relationship between the predictors and the expected outcomes.

Further discussion of the components of this proposed framework follows below, with subsequent descriptions of the empirical evidence supporting each component. Early childhood health risk factors assumed to increase the risk of childhood externalizing behavior and adult violence include exposure to alcohol, tobacco, toxicity and drugs during pregnancy; head injury during early childhood; teenage pregnancy; birth complications; and malnutrition both of the pregnant mother and the child in early postnatal years. This empirical review does not aim to cover all these health factors and will instead focus on those of greatest relevance to the proposed framework (summarized in Table 1).

2.2 Overview of Components of the Framework

2.2.1 Biological Risk Factors

During the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods, pertinent biological risk factors include both genetic and maternal pathophysiological processes that can impede fetal growth and development (Liu & Wuerker, 2005). While many early biological risk factors during the pre-and postnatal period may be linked to childhood externalizing behaviors, subsequent sections briefly review only a few of the most important factors, including maternal malnutrition, smoking during pregnancy, and birth complications.

2.2.2 Psychosocial Risk Factors

Psychosocial risk factors have been hypothesized to be associated with an increased risk for negative behavioral outcomes. Research on psychosocial risk factors has focused on child abuse (Widom, 1989), marital conflict (Margolin, 1988), poor parenting (Hodgins et al., 2001), poor child rearing (Webster-Stratton & Hammond 1999), domestic violence (Stoff et al., 1997), intimate partner violence (Campbell et al., 2007), and community violence (Scarpa & Haden, 2006). Other important considerations include low socioeconomic status, high psychosocial stress, negative attitudes about pregnancy, pregnancy during adolescence, and psychiatric disorders including substance abuse.

2.2.3 Protective Factors (Prevention and Intervention)

In this framework, protective factors are proposed to be introduced as early as before health risk factors are developed, and can serve to reduce biological vulnerability and intervene psychosocial influence each individually as well as to break down the interaction effects which produce early health risk factors. Protective factors increase one’s resilience, enhance resistance to risk, and strengthen individual defenses against the development of negative outcomes. The prenatal developmental period is intuitively an important time in which to focus on preventative measures because the developing fetus is highly vulnerable to influences from the environment such as maternal nutrition, toxins, and other health-related variables. Thus, proper prenatal care, including regular physical examinations, proper nutrition, general healthy lifestyle, avoidance of toxic chemicals (e.g., tobacco and substance use), as well as strong parental bonding and family harmony, are vital for risk reduction and increasing the chance of a positive outcome despite risk exposure.

2.2.4 Health Risk Factors: The Interaction between Biology and the Psychosocial Environment

Etiology of child externalizing behavior, delinquency, and adult violence is complex, and it is likely that both genes and environment contribute (Baker et al., 2008). An interaction refers to the effects of two risk factors are not merely additive, but are instead multiplicative. The framework argues that the likelihood of early health factors leading to later externalizing behavior would be strongly increased when biological risk factors combine with social risk factors. For example, birth complications, an early health risk factor which has been repeated found to predispose children to later violence (Liu et al., 2009) can be due to both biological risk factors (e.g. poor nutrition) and psychosocial risk factors (e.g. lack of prenatal care). The argument that psychosocial and biological factors interact to influence behavior is also support by other studies. Maternal cigarette smoking has been found to interact with paternal cigarette smoking to increase the risk of juvenile offending (Gibson & Tibbetts, 2000). Rasanen et al. (1999) found that maternal smoking combined with social risk factors (teenage pregnancy, single family status, and unwanted pregnancy), increased the odds ratios for violent offending by up to 9-fold for non-persistent offenses and up to 14-fold for persistent offenses. Furthermore, Caspi et al. (2002) also found that maltreated children with a genotype conferring high levels of monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A) expression were less likely to develop antisocial problems. Kim-Cohen et al. (2006) then further reported that mental health problems are significantly stronger in males with the genotype conferring low MAOA activity.

Recently, epigenetics - the study of inherited alterations in gene expression brought about by mechanisms other than changes in the underlying DNA sequence – has gained increasing attention in health risk factor research as recognition of multiple influences on maternal-fetal pathways has emerged. Epigenetic factors appear important in predicting behavioral outcomes as they may impinge on the neurodevelopment of the fetus’ growing brain. For example, exposure to environmental chemicals during the prenatal period may affect gene transcription, which subsequently influences the development of epigenetic disorders (Fushiki, 2009). Given that biology and the social environment are intertwined and may even have synergistic effects as opposed to independent effects, they are collectively defined unitarily as “health risk factors”.

This framework also recognizes the reciprocal interaction between biological and psychosocial risk factors. The reciprocal interaction is thus defined because psychosocial factors can give rise to biological risk factors, just as biological risk factors can predispose certain individuals to social risk factors. For example, teenage pregnancy as one component of social adversity is generally viewed as a psychosocial risk factor, but it could be that genetic/biological traits (e.g. high hormone levels) may predispose some teenager to engage in sexual activities and become pregnant. Another example of reciprocal interaction is that substance abuse is often viewed as a social-behavioral problem (Curran et al., 2000), but it could also be argued that individuals who abuse drugs and alcohol have a genetic/biological vulnerability to such behavior (Raine et al., 1997).

Thus, this framework proposes that early health risk factors, which results from the interaction between biological and psychosocial risk factors, are critically important in predisposition of later aggression, hyperactivity, and delinquency.

2.2.5 The Mediating Factor of the Brain

Related to the concept of interactive effects, resulting in early health risk factors, is mediating effects, which help explain the mechanism of action when an outcome is observed. Low IQ is among the most-replicated cognitive mediating factors for behavior problems (Hirachi & Hindelang, 1977; Moffitt, 1990). As discussed previously, findings from Liu et al. (2009) document that low IQ mediates the relationship between birth complications and externalizing behaviors. Other authors have similarly found that low IQ mediates the relationship between prenatal biological factors and externalizing outcomes, including such factors as nutrition (Liu, 2004; Liu et al., 2005), maternal tobacco exposure (Weitzman et al., 2002), and maternal alcohol use (Korkman et al., 2003). Furthermore, longitudinal studies have suggested that neuropsychological deficits characterize child and adult violence and antisocial behavior (Raine, 2002), and early neurocognitive deficits precede the onset of these antisocial behaviors (Moffitt, 1990).

Spatial IQ is defined as the non-verbal ability to perform complex visuo-spatial tasks (such as putting blocks together to form a pre-specified pattern, work one’s way through a maze) and is related to problem solving abilities in mathematics, engineering, architecture, science, art, and games. Low spatial IQ at age 3, as well as low verbal and spatial IQ at age 11, predicted Life-Course-Persistent antisocial behavior in community children (Raine et al., 2002). Similar results, showing that impairments in spatial abilities, but not verbal abilities, have been identified among antisocial children (Spelz et al., 1999). A possible mechanism by which low IQ in general functions as a mediating factor could be through its numerous psychosocial influences, including decreased bonding with parents, school failure, occupational failure, and low self-esteem – all of which increase the likelihood for antisocial behavior (Liu et al., 2010).

Regarding specific components of intelligence, verbal IQ on the widely-used Wechsler scales has been more strongly correlated with achievement than performance IQ (Kaufman & Lichtenberger, 2006). The predictive relationship between low verbal intelligence and later criminal behavior may reflect low academic achievement and school difficulties in general.

The relationship between spatial abilities and antisocial behavior may be due to right hemispheric damage, which would result in visuospatial deficits. Right hemisphere dominance aids in facial recognition and orientation, attention, and arousal in infants and young children. Impairment to this brain hemisphere could hinder child-parent bonding and affective processing and regulation (Raine et al., 2005). As such, in this proposed framework cognition is hypothesized to mediate the biosocial risk on externalizing and violent behaviors.

2.2.6 Impact of Early Health Risk Factors on Neurodevelopment and Subsequent Behaviors

Raine (2002) and Raine et al. (2002) present evidence from brain imaging, neuropsychological, and neurological studies that uniformly suggest damage or dysfunction to the prefrontal cortex as a significant predisposition to antisocial behavior. Low IQ and neurocognitive deficits manifested in the right hemisphere, and specifically in the visual and motor cortices, appear particularly important. These deficits can impair a child’s ability to establish visual and physical connections with the parents, thereby reducing parental bonding. Individuals who show significant instrumental aggression have shown evidence of amygdala dysfunction, resulting in resistance to aversive conditioning and passive avoidance learning (Blair, 2004).

The processes that can induce neurocognitive deficits are numerous. Sowell et al. (2007)collected functional MRI data showing that children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure exhibit functional abnormalities in the left medial temporal and dorsal prefrontal lobes and difficulties, areas which are pertinent in verbal learning. De Haan et al. (2006) as well as others (Sie et al., 2000) have put forth imaging evidence that perinatal asphyxia in term birth infants can lead to damage to the brainstem, thalamus, basal ganglia, parasagittal areas, and hippocampus. In an earlier study by Robertson et al. (1989), among infants who survived peri-natal asphyxia 25% showed significant neurological impairments, including encephalopathy, which has been correlated with impaired general intellectual abilities, slower academic progress, and poorer visual-motor integration in school-aged children. Non-accidental head injury, including shaken-baby syndrome, can cause diffuse neurological insult particularly to the prefrontal cortex. It is no surprise therefore that these injuries are significantly associated with a range of negative neurological outcomes, including a greater incidence of convulsions and subsequent hypoxia; sensory processing and motor deficits; language difficulties; and memory, attention, and other executive dysfunction (Ashton, 2010). Of note, infants and very young children appear to be especially prone to these negative outcomes (Ashton, 2010). Bellinger et al (2008) reports that lead exposure may preferentially inhibit functioning in several areas of the brain, including the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, postcentral gyri, inferior parietal lobule, and cerebellum. More recently, brain imaging studies show that exposure to lead during childhood is associated with decreased gray matter volume in adulthood, especially in portions of the prefrontal cortex which are responsible for mood regulation and decision-making (Cecil et al., 2008), which in turn predisposes these individuals to violent and criminal behavior. Consequently, lead exposure has been associated with outcomes related to poor impulse control, including violent offending, substance use, and ADHD (Bellinger et al., 2008).

3. Empirical Evidence of Health Risk Factors during Prenatal and Early Childhood

The following section briefly reviews the current empirical evidence supporting components of this framework. This includes the following health risk factors: prenatal maternal smoking; malnutrition; lead exposure; birth complications; childhood head injury; maternal depression and stress; and child abuse. While these do not exhaustively represent all health risk factors of importance to behavioural outcomes, they reflect factors that are most salient, are well-supported by the literature and, to a large degree are amenable to prevention and intervention strategies.

3.1 Smoking during Pregnancy

There is increasing evidence recognizing a significant association between smoking during pregnancy and later conduct disorder and violent offending (Ashford et al., 2008; D’Onofrio et al., 2010; Gibson & Tibbetts, 2000; Raine, 2002; Rasanen et al., 1999, Wakschlag et al., 2009), even after controlling for demographic, parental, and perinatal risk confounds (Brennan et al, 1999, 2002,). In a large birth cohort of males (N = 4169), Brennan et al. (1999) found a two-fold increase in adult violent offending among offspring of mothers who smoked 20 or more cigarettes per day during pregnancy, with an increasing dose-response relationship between number of cigarettes smoked and rates of violence. Furthermore, a recent study has shown that even second-hand exposure to cigarette smoking in the mother has been associated with increased externalizing psychopathology in the child (Gatzke-Kopp & Beauchaine, 2007). The exact mechanism through which maternal tobacco exposure predisposes to behavioral problems in offspring is unclear. However, carbon monoxide and nicotine have been implicated in numerous areas of declining neurofunctioning, including noradrenergic disruption and serotonin and dopamine regulation (Muneoka et al., 1997); reductions in brain glucose (Eckstein et al., 1997); and basal ganglia, cerebral cortex, and cerebellar cortex dysfunction (Olds, 1997; Raine, 2002).

3.2 Malnutrition

Nutrition factors play an active and critical role in brain development during pregnancy and early childhood. Human studies suggest that a deficiency in macronutrients (e.g. protein), micronutrients (e.g. the trace elements zinc and iron), and a component of omega-3 fatty acids (docosahexaenoic acid or DHA – a long-chain essential fatty acid) can disturb brain functioning and thus further predispose to behavior disorders in children (Breakey, 1997; Fishbein, 2001; Lister et al., 2005; Liu & Raine, 2006; Werbach, 1992). Neugebauer et al., (1999) found that the male offspring of mothers severely malnourished during the first and second trimesters of pregnancy had 2.5 times the normal rate of antisocial personality disorder in adulthood. Postnatally, prospective findings indicate that children with protein, zinc, iron, and vitamin B deficiencies at age 3 years exhibited greater antisocial, aggressive, and/or hyperactive behaviors at ages 8, 11, and 17 years and the effects are mediated by cognitive deficits (Liu et al., 2004; Liu et al., 2003). Zinc deficiency has been specifically associated with greater violence and aggression (Liu et al., 2004; Watts, 1990) as well as hyperactivity (Brophy, 1986). In animal models, monkeys and rats deprived of the amino acid tryptophan show higher rates of aggression (Bjork et al., 1999), possibly due to the role of tryptophan in synthesizing serotonin, which has been shown to be reduced in individuals with impulsivity and violent behaviors (Virkkunmen et al., 1995; Werbach, 1995). Low iron has also been linked to aggression, conduct disorder, and juvenile delinquency (Rosen et al., 1985; Werbach, 1995). Furthermore, iron supplementation was shown improve cognitive and behavioral outcomes among non-iron deficient children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Sever et al., 1997).

The precise mechanism of action in these relationships is not well understood. There is speculation that proteins or minerals play a role in either regulating neurotransmitters or hormones, or else exacerbate neurotoxins, and in doing so predispose to brain dysfunction, aggression, and hyperactivity (Coccaro et al., 1997; Ferris & Grisso, 1996; Liu & Raine, 2006). Despite these promising findings, the research literature on malnutrition and externalizing behaviors remains both limited and controversial (Liu et al., 2004).

3.3 Lead Exposure

The toxic effects of lead on neurodevelopment are well-known, along with their consequent impact on behavior and development (Chen & Rogan, 2005; Dietrich et al., 2004; Koller et al., 2004; Needleman et al., 1996; Bellinger et al., 2005). Both animal studies (Delville, 1999) and human studies have found that higher lead levels measured either in the bone or blood are significantly related to aggressive and delinquent behavior in childhood (Burns et al., 1999; Needleman et al., 1996) as well as in early adulthood (Wright et al., 2008). Monkeys exposed to lead show patterns of behavioral impairment resembling the deficits of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Rice, 2000). Burns et al. (1999) found that school children from a lead-smelting community had increased externalizing behavior problems. This effect remained after controlling for a number of confounding variables, including quality of the child’s home environment, maternal psychopathology, and the child’s IQ. Sciarillo et al. (1992) found that children with elevated blood lead had behavior problems scores that were 2.7 times those of the controls. In addition, even children with low lead exposure may suffer from impaired cognitive performance (Koller et al., 2004). For example, Needleman (1996)found that children with elevated bone lead levels but no symptoms of lead toxicity showed higher aggression and delinquency scores compared to low-lead counterparts (Needleman, 1996). More recently, Wright et al. (2008) found that elevated blood lead concentrations during the prenatal and postnatal are associated with higher rates of criminal arrests in early adulthood. Nevertheless, studies in this area are to date few and limited, with many studies limited by small sample size or restriction to males only.

It is recognized that elevated blood lead concentrations are more common among children living in poverty, likely accounting for the mediating role of socioeconomic status between lead and behavioral outcomes (ATSDR, 1992; Hubbs-Tait et al., 2005). Environmental toxicity, micronutrients deficiency, and negative social environments may all produce adverse cognitive and behavioral outcomes (Moore, 2005), underscoring the importance of recognizing combined “health risk factors” (i.e. early health, biological, and psychosocial factors).

3.4 Birth Complications

Birth complications have been associated with antisocial behavior, particularly in the context of mitigating factors such as socioeconomic status and family stress. A large sample (N=4,269) of Danish males demonstrated an association between birth complications and violent behavior in adulthood when combined with early maternal rejection of the child (Raine et al., 1994). This has been replicated in four other international samples (Sweden, Finland, Canada, and United States). Prospective longitudinal research has similarly linked obstetric complications in the context of disadvantaged familial environment to increased adult violent crime (Piquero & Tibbetts, 1999). The interaction between pregnancy complications and poor parenting also predicts to adult violence (Hodgins, Kratzer, & Mcneil, 2001), as does the interaction between increased serious obstetric complications and family adversity (Arsenault et al., 2002). For instance, in a sample of Finnish males (N=5,587), the interaction between perinatal risk and being an only child raised the odds of adult violent offending by a factor of 4.4 (Kemppainen et al., 2001).

Recent evidence suggests that cognition might represent a possible third variable accounting for the relationship between birth complications and later externalizing behavior. Liu et al. (2009) examined the influence of birth complications in predisposing to externalizing behaviors at age 11 years. Birth complications included prenatal factors such as hypertension, low blood albumin, bleeding, and other illness (e.g., diabetes, pneumonia, asthma, and epilepsy). Perinatal factors consisted of breech fetal positioning, premature rupture of membrane, fetal distress, and use of instrument delivery (e.g., forceps, Cesarean section). Postnatal factors included cyanosis and treatment with oxygen. Among a sample of nearly 1,800 children, low IQ was significantly associated with both birth complications and externalizing behavior and was found to mediate the birth complications-externalizing behavior relationship. Birth complications may directly contribute to brain dysfunction by impeding proper neurodevelopment. For example, hypoxia selectively damages the hippocampal formation, thereby impairing the limbic system, a brain area repeatedly documented by brain imaging research to be associated with aggression regulation and disinhibition (Liu & Wuerker, 2005; Raine, 2002).

3.5 Head Injury

The literature linking even minor head injuries to violent behavior has consistently implicated traumatic brain injury in children in increasing emotional dysregulation and behavioral problems, including externalizing behaviors (Hatzitaskos et al., 1994; Andrews et al. 1998; Hatzitaskos et al., 1994; Max et al., 1998). Findings from neuropsychological assessments indicate significantly greater frontal and temporal dysfunction among antisocial individuals as compared to controls (Moffitt, 1990). Neuropsychological impairment may in part be a function of poor maternal prenatal conditions (e.g., alcohol and tobacco exposure). Furthermore, brain imaging studies have found that aggressive prisoners exhibit significantly reduced glucose metabolism in the prefrontal cortex relative to matched controls (Raine et al., 1997a). While findings are variable, antisocial and violent individuals as a group appear to be more likely to demonstrate neurocognitive deficits relative to non-violent controls.

3.6 Maternal Depression

Maternal depression has been consistently found to increase the potential for negative behaviors in the offspring (Hoffman et al., 2006; Phelan et al., 2007). In one sample, maternal depression was associated with a six-fold increase in the risk for childhood externalizing behavior (Mohan et al., 1998). More recently, a large-scale longitudinal study found that maternal depression was significantly associated with externalizing behavior boys at the age of 24 months, but by the time the children entered first grade, the relationship was stronger amongst girls (Blatt-Eisengart et al., 2009). This is consistent with other researchers who have noted a moderating effect of gender on the relationship between maternal depression and externalizing behavior (Burt et al., 2005; Wall and Holden, 1994). The contribution of maternal mental health on childhood aggression may be mediated by family factors, such as intra-family conflict and degree of spousal support (Malik et al., 2007) or other maternal characteristics, such as hostile and controlling behaviors (Marchand et al., 2002).

3.7 Maternal Stress

Although not outlined in the proposed framework, maternal stress should also be recognized as an important, overarching component of health risk factors as it can result from any of the above risk factors. Prenatal maternal stress is viewed as exposure of a pregnant woman to distress - either psychosocial or environmental - which induces biochemical changes in the mother that subsequently adversely affect the fetus’ growing organ systems, including the brain. As a multifaceted factor, prenatal stress has been studied in various forms, such as chronic anxiety (Sjostrom et al., 1997) and psychosocial stress (Entringer et al., 2009). Associations have been found between prenatal stress and low birth weight (Wadhwa et al., 2001). Additionally, findings from animal studies have linked prenatal stress to neurodevelopmental modifications in rats (Glover et al., 2009). However, while recent research has demonstrated an association between prenatal stress and the developing fetus (particularly the development of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal, or HPA, axis, - Glover et al., 2009) the link between prenatal stress and behavior remains largely unresolved.

Prenatal stress can be measured through biomarkers, such as cortisol level measurements, or through self-report, such as the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Maternal anxiety as measured by the STAI was thought to be related to a down regulation in placental 11β-HSD2, an enzyme that protects the fetus from high levels of circulating maternal cortisol (Glover et al., 2009). High cortisol levels are associated with adverse birth outcomes and may alter fetal development and subsequent adult health (Evans et al., 2008). Furthermore, prenatal stress can be denoted retrospectively. Study shows that experiencing a major negative life event was found to be associated with an alteration in the fetus’ hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning (Entringer, et al., 2009). Such changes are associated with physical and mental disorders, and the study authors concluded that psychosocial stress has a strong impact on an offspring’s mental and behavioral development. Prenatal stress, including exposure to toxicants such as cocaine during pregnancy, has been shown to be linked to elevated salivary cortisol stress reactivity in school-aged children (Lester et al., 2010), and Susman et al. (2010) found that high cortisol reactivity in early adolescent children was directly related to antisocial and rule- breaking behavior.

3.8 Child Abuse

It is not surprising that child abuse and neglect are strongly associated with a host of negative outcomes, including behavioral disturbance in adolescence and later adulthood (Widom, 1989; Teicher, 2000). Abused and neglected children were nearly five times as likely to be arrested for juvenile delinquency, with Caucasian youth demonstrating a staggering 20-fold increase in risk for juvenile arrest for violent crime (English et al., 2002). Moreover, the odds of arrest for criminal behavior as an adult were twice as high amongst victims of abuse. These trends were demonstrated in both males and females. Child maltreatment has also been linked to

Link to Article

Abstract

Violence and aggression are public health problems that can benefit from ongoing research into risk reduction and prevention. Current developmental theories of violence and aggression emphasize biological and psychosocial factors, particularly during adolescence. However, there has been less focus on understanding the interactive, multiplicative effects of these processes. Furthermore, little attention has been given to the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods, where prevention and intervention may yield effective results. Early health risk factors that influence negative behavioral outcomes include prenatal and postnatal nutrition, tobacco use during pregnancy, maternal depression, birth complications, traumatic brain injury, lead exposure, and child abuse. There is an ample literature to suggest that these early health risk factors may increase the likelihood of childhood externalizing behaviors, aggression, juvenile delinquency, adult criminal behavior, and/or violence. This paper proposes an early health risk factors framework for violence prediction, built on existing developmental theories of criminal behavior and supported by empirical findings. This framework addresses gaps in the adolescent psychopathology literature and presents a novel conceptualization of behavioral disturbance that emphasizes the pre-, peri-, and post-natal periods, when a child’s development is critical and the opportunity for behavioral and environmental modification is high. Implications for such a framework on violence prevention programs are discussed.

Summary

This paper proposes a new conceptual framework for understanding and preventing violence based on early childhood health risk factors. It examines how biological and psychosocial factors interact to influence behavior, with a particular emphasis on the brain as a mediating factor. The paper outlines several key health risk factors, including prenatal maternal smoking, malnutrition, lead exposure, birth complications, childhood head injury, maternal depression and stress, and child abuse. The paper argues that addressing these factors early in life, through prevention programs and interventions, can help reduce the incidence of violence and aggression in children and adolescents.

1. Introduction

Childhood aggression, teenage delinquency, and adult violent acts are increasingly viewed as a public health issue. Identifying early risk factors is a critical step in preventing violence, a major societal problem. Despite decades of research into social and biological risk factors for antisocial and aggressive behavior in children and adults, very little is known about the effects of early childhood health factors on these outcomes. Specifically, research into the causes of antisocial and violent behavior has been hindered by three key gaps in the literature: empirical, conceptual, and methodological.

1.1 Gaps in the Current Literature

The main empirical gap is the relative neglect of the role of early childhood health risk factors, such as prenatal factors, in the child and adolescent psychopathology literature. Recent research suggests that the prenatal stage of child development may be a key period for effective interventions to potentially prevent later aggressive and violent behavior. However, there is a dearth of literature explicitly discussing the unique impact of prenatal contributors to these outcomes.

The second gap is conceptual: theoretical frameworks of early risk factors for violence and aggression have not effectively integrated social and biological factors. Furthermore, health risk factors have been largely ignored from these frameworks. One exception is Raine et al. (1997), who proposed a biosocial interaction model for understanding antisocial behavior. Violent behavior in adults and externalizing behavior in children are complex phenomena, and it is likely that neither biological nor psychosocial perspectives alone can adequately explain these outcomes. Considering early health factors along with biosocial processes provides a multi-perspective, cross-disciplinary approach that may improve the empirical foundation of the literature and lead to the development of more effective interventions.

The third gap is methodological: there is a lack of prospective longitudinal studies on early health risk factors for negative behavioral outcomes. Past studies on childhood antisocial and aggressive behavior were mainly correlational and based on cross-sectional study designs or retrospective investigations. Prospective, longitudinal studies, while still correlational, can help tease out the temporal ordering of variables and assess whether the data are consistent with a causal model, particularly when structural equation modeling is used.

1.2 Current Theoretical Models and the Need for a New Approach

Some of the most important research on developmental theories of violence and aggression has emanated from the work of Moffitt, Farrington, and Fergusson. Moffitt's dual pathway developmental theory of criminal behavior categorizes antisocial adolescents according to their age of onset of deviant behavior (i.e., “early starters” versus “late starters”). “Early starters” are more likely to develop into life-course persistent offenders rather than adolescent-limited offenders (as with “late starters”). Individuals in the LCP group are characterized by a combination of temperamental (e.g., negative emotionality) and neurological (e.g., inattention, hyperactivity, low IQ, low resting heart rate) antecedents that begin early in life and give rise to deviant behavior that steadily continues throughout adolescence, especially when combined with environmental deficits such as poor parenting.

Farrington's integrated cognitive antisocial potential theory parallels Moffitt’s framework in that it too emphasizes the predictability and developmental trajectories of criminal behavior, with particular attention paid to temperamental and neurological influences. His theory considers an individual’s antisocial potential (AP), or likelihood of engaging in antisocial behaviors, as well as cognitive thought processes that predispose to antisocial behavior. Farrington differentiates individuals with long-term AP from those with short-term AP, much like Moffitt’s LCP and adolescent-persistent distinction. Similar to Moffitt’s findings among LCP individuals, Farrington has reported that individuals with long-term AP are more likely to come from unstable family environments, exhibit lower IQ, display poor attention and hyperactivity, and have poor socialization skills.

While research from Moffitt, Farrington, and Fergusson has been influential, there is room for improvement. The children in Moffitt’s studies were all over 3 years of age. Similarly, Farrington’s samples were ages 10 years and older, and Fergusson’s samples were all ages 8 years and older. The theoretical model presented in this paper - the early health risk factor model - builds upon the work of these researchers by taking an integrated approach that includes health, biological, and psychosocial variables to better understanding predictors of negative behaviors, but it is unique in its application to early childhood. It is specifically designed to focus on the early childhood trajectory years and beyond) and developmental outcomes, and in this way can be conceptualized as an extension of Moffitt’s LCP model. Second, while Moffitt’s and Farrington’s models recognize the importance of biological as well as psychosocial factors, they do not necessarily emphasize the interactive effects between these variables. This is an important consideration, as possible synergistic effects may further increase the risk for negative outcomes beyond that of each of the risk factors independently. This framework addresses this oversight by proposing a greater focus on the interrelationships between a child’s biological and environmental background.

The field may benefit from a conceptual framework that both supports replicated findings about violence and aggression risk factors in children and adolescents while at the same time addressing the above described gaps, thus providing a relatively new perspective on understanding, predicting, and preventing these outcomes. This paper therefore introduces a new framework of early childhood health risk factors that stresses the multiplicative effects of biological, psychosocial, and environmental factors. The focus on early childhood health risk factors also helps inform the development of violence prevention programs, which may reduce childhood behaviors and possible prevent their escalation into more serious juvenile delinquency and adult crime. This framework is presented below, followed by a brief review of recent empirical finding to support this proposal.

2. An Early Health Conceptual Framework for Violence Study

2.1 Overview of the Conceptual Framework

The World Health Organization (WHO) defines health risk factors as behaviors and conditions that increase the likelihood for major injury, disease, or death. Among the 20 health risk factors that the WHO identifies as being of primary importance, preventable health risk factors include low childhood and maternal weight, tobacco and alcohol use, iron deficiency, obesity, high cholesterol and blood pressure, and unsafe sexual practices. These factors have considerable impact on longevity and quality of life, as worldwide estimates suggest they contribute to more than 29 million deaths annually.

This proposal for an early health risk factor framework is built upon the author’s previous model of child externalizing behavior, which was derived from a biosocial model of violence. In this proposed new framework, health risk factors are defined as byproducts of biological and psychosocial processes during the pre-, peri-, and postnatal period and are hypothesized to be predictors of adverse outcomes, including childhood externalizing behavior, juvenile delinquency, and adult violence. Of note, the early health risk factors outlined in this framework focuses only on those which occur during prenatal period and childhood, are known to impair brain functions, and which would consequently be expected to influence behavior. Specifically, more negative behavioral conditions are hypothesized to be derived from prenatal insults or toxins, including fetal alcohol syndrome, and emerge on the basis of neural maldevelopment that results in cognitive dysfunction, in turn predisposing to life-course-persistent anti-social behavior. As a result, not all early health risk factors can be exhaustively encompassed since some are still lacking empirical evidence demonstrating their relationship to neurological deficits and violent/aggressive behaviors. In this framework, biological factors by themselves are hypothesized to directly give rise to behavioral outcomes. Similarly, social risk factors can independently and directly lead to externalizing behaviors. Importantly, the framework emphasizes that the interaction between the biological and psychosocial risk factors, which develops early health risk factors, along with potential mediators, may account for the relationship between the predictors and the expected outcomes.

Early childhood health risk factors assumed to increase the risk of childhood externalizing behavior and adult violence include exposure to alcohol, tobacco, toxicity and drugs during pregnancy; head injury during early childhood; teenage pregnancy; birth complications; and malnutrition both of the pregnant mother and the child in early postnatal years. This empirical review does not aim to cover all these health factors and will instead focus on those of greatest relevance to the proposed framework (summarized in Table 1).

2.2 Overview of Components of the Framework

2.2.1 Biological Risk Factors

During the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods, pertinent biological risk factors include both genetic and maternal pathophysiological processes that can impede fetal growth and development. While many early biological risk factors during the pre-and postnatal period may be linked to childhood externalizing behaviors, subsequent sections briefly review only a few of the most important factors, including maternal malnutrition, smoking during pregnancy, and birth complications.

2.2.2 Psychosocial Risk Factors

Psychosocial risk factors have been hypothesized to be associated with an increased risk for negative behavioral outcomes. Research on psychosocial risk factors has focused on child abuse, marital conflict, poor parenting, poor child rearing, domestic violence, intimate partner violence, and community violence. Other important considerations include low socioeconomic status, high psychosocial stress, negative attitudes about pregnancy, pregnancy during adolescence, and psychiatric disorders including substance abuse.

2.2.3 Protective Factors (Prevention and Intervention)

In this framework, protective factors are proposed to be introduced as early as before health risk factors are developed, and can serve to reduce biological vulnerability and intervene psychosocial influence each individually as well as to break down the interaction effects which produce early health risk factors. Protective factors increase one’s resilience, enhance resistance to risk, and strengthen individual defenses against the development of negative outcomes. The prenatal developmental period is intuitively an important time in which to focus on preventative measures because the developing fetus is highly vulnerable to influences from the environment such as maternal nutrition, toxins, and other health-related variables. Thus, proper prenatal care, including regular physical examinations, proper nutrition, general healthy lifestyle, avoidance of toxic chemicals (e.g., tobacco and substance use), as well as strong parental bonding and family harmony, are vital for risk reduction and increasing the chance of a positive outcome despite risk exposure.

2.2.4 Health Risk Factors: The Interaction between Biology and the Psychosocial Environment

Etiology of child externalizing behavior, delinquency, and adult violence is complex, and it is likely that both genes and environment contribute. An interaction refers to the effects of two risk factors are not merely additive, but are instead multiplicative. The framework argues that the likelihood of early health factors leading to later externalizing behavior would be strongly increased when biological risk factors combine with social risk factors. For example, birth complications, an early health risk factor which has been repeated found to predispose children to later violence, can be due to both biological risk factors (e.g. poor nutrition) and psychosocial risk factors (e.g. lack of prenatal care). The argument that psychosocial and biological factors interact to influence behavior is also support by other studies. Maternal cigarette smoking has been found to interact with paternal cigarette smoking to increase the risk of juvenile offending.

Recently, epigenetics - the study of inherited alterations in gene expression brought about by mechanisms other than changes in the underlying DNA sequence – has gained increasing attention in health risk factor research as recognition of multiple influences on maternal-fetal pathways has emerged. Epigenetic factors appear important in predicting behavioral outcomes as they may impinge on the neurodevelopment of the fetus’ growing brain. For example, exposure to environmental chemicals during the prenatal period may affect gene transcription, which subsequently influences the development of epigenetic disorders. Given that biology and the social environment are intertwined and may even have synergistic effects as opposed to independent effects, they are collectively defined unitarily as “health risk factors”.

This framework also recognizes the reciprocal interaction between biological and psychosocial risk factors. The reciprocal interaction is thus defined because psychosocial factors can give rise to biological risk factors, just as biological risk factors can predispose certain individuals to social risk factors. For example, teenage pregnancy as one component of social adversity is generally viewed as a psychosocial risk factor, but it could be that genetic/biological traits (e.g. high hormone levels) may predispose some teenager to engage in sexual activities and become pregnant. Another example of reciprocal interaction is that substance abuse is often viewed as a social-behavioral problem, but it could also be argued that individuals who abuse drugs and alcohol have a genetic/biological vulnerability to such behavior.

Thus, this framework proposes that early health risk factors, which results from the interaction between biological and psychosocial risk factors, are critically important in predisposition of later aggression, hyperactivity, and delinquency.

2.2.5 The Mediating Factor of the Brain

Related to the concept of interactive effects, resulting in early health risk factors, is mediating effects, which help explain the mechanism of action when an outcome is observed. Low IQ is among the most-replicated cognitive mediating factors for behavior problems. As discussed previously, findings from Liu et al. (2009) document that low IQ mediates the relationship between birth complications and externalizing behaviors. Other authors have similarly found that low IQ mediates the relationship between prenatal biological factors and externalizing outcomes, including such factors as nutrition, maternal tobacco exposure, and maternal alcohol use. Furthermore, longitudinal studies have suggested that neuropsychological deficits characterize child and adult violence and antisocial behavior, and early neurocognitive deficits precede the onset of these antisocial behaviors.

2.2.6 Impact of Early Health Risk Factors on Neurodevelopment and Subsequent Behaviors

Raine (2002) and Raine et al. (2002) present evidence from brain imaging, neuropsychological, and neurological studies that uniformly suggest damage or dysfunction to the prefrontal cortex as a significant predisposition to antisocial behavior. Low IQ and neurocognitive deficits manifested in the right hemisphere, and specifically in the visual and motor cortices, appear particularly important. These deficits can impair a child’s ability to establish visual and physical connections with the parents, thereby reducing parental bonding. Individuals who show significant instrumental aggression have shown evidence of amygdala dysfunction, resulting in resistance to aversive conditioning and passive avoidance learning.

The processes that can induce neurocognitive deficits are numerous. Sowell et al. (2007) collected functional MRI data showing that children with heavy prenatal alcohol exposure exhibit functional abnormalities in the left medial temporal and dorsal prefrontal lobes and difficulties, areas which are pertinent in verbal learning. De Haan et al. (2006) as well as others have put forth imaging evidence that perinatal asphyxia in term birth infants can lead to damage to the brainstem, thalamus, basal ganglia, parasagittal areas, and hippocampus. Non-accidental head injury, including shaken-baby syndrome, can cause diffuse neurological insult particularly to the prefrontal cortex. It is no surprise therefore that these injuries are significantly associated with a range of negative neurological outcomes, including a greater incidence of convulsions and subsequent hypoxia; sensory processing and motor deficits; language difficulties; and memory, attention, and other executive dysfunction. Of note, infants and very young children appear to be especially prone to these negative outcomes. Bellinger et al (2008) reports that lead exposure may preferentially inhibit functioning in several areas of the brain, including the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, postcentral gyri, inferior parietal lobule, and cerebellum. More recently, brain imaging studies show that exposure to lead during childhood is associated with decreased gray matter volume in adulthood, especially in portions of the prefrontal cortex which are responsible for mood regulation and decision-making, which in turn predisposes these individuals to violent and criminal behavior. Consequently, lead exposure has been associated with outcomes related to poor impulse control, including violent offending, substance use, and ADHD.

3. Empirical Evidence of Health Risk Factors during Prenatal and Early Childhood

The following section briefly reviews the current empirical evidence supporting components of this framework. This includes the following health risk factors: prenatal maternal smoking; malnutrition; lead exposure; birth complications; childhood head injury; maternal depression and stress; and child abuse. While these do not exhaustively represent all health risk factors of importance to behavioural outcomes, they reflect factors that are most salient, are well-supported by the literature and, to a large degree are amenable to prevention and intervention strategies.

3.1 Smoking during Pregnancy

There is increasing evidence recognizing a significant association between smoking during pregnancy and later conduct disorder and violent offending. In a large birth cohort of males, Brennan et al. (1999) found a two-fold increase in adult violent offending among offspring of mothers who smoked 20 or more cigarettes per day during pregnancy, with an increasing dose-response relationship between number of cigarettes smoked and rates of violence. Furthermore, a recent study has shown that even second-hand exposure to cigarette smoking in the mother has been associated with increased externalizing psychopathology in the child. The exact mechanism through which maternal tobacco exposure predisposes to behavioral problems in offspring is unclear. However, carbon monoxide and nicotine have been implicated in numerous areas of declining neurofunctioning, including noradrenergic disruption and serotonin and dopamine regulation; reductions in brain glucose; and basal ganglia, cerebral cortex, and cerebellar cortex dysfunction.

3.2 Malnutrition

Nutrition factors play an active and critical role in brain development during pregnancy and early childhood. Human studies suggest that a deficiency in macronutrients (e.g. protein), micronutrients (e.g. the trace elements zinc and iron), and a component of omega-3 fatty acids (docosahexaenoic acid or DHA – a long-chain essential fatty acid) can disturb brain functioning and thus further predispose to behavior disorders in children. Neugebauer et al., (1999) found that the male offspring of mothers severely malnourished during the first and second trimesters of pregnancy had 2.5 times the normal rate of antisocial personality disorder in adulthood. Postnatally, prospective findings indicate that children with protein, zinc, iron, and vitamin B deficiencies at age 3 years exhibited greater antisocial, aggressive, and/or hyperactive behaviors at ages 8, 11, and 17 years and the effects are mediated by cognitive deficits. Zinc deficiency has been specifically associated with greater violence and aggression as well as hyperactivity. In animal models, monkeys and rats deprived of the amino acid tryptophan show higher rates of aggression, possibly due to the role of tryptophan in synthesizing serotonin, which has been shown to be reduced in individuals with impulsivity and violent behaviors. Low iron has also been linked to aggression, conduct disorder, and juvenile delinquency. Furthermore, iron supplementation was shown improve cognitive and behavioral outcomes among non-iron deficient children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.

The precise mechanism of action in these relationships is not well understood. There is speculation that proteins or minerals play a role in either regulating neurotransmitters or hormones, or else exacerbate neurotoxins, and in doing so predispose to brain dysfunction, aggression, and hyperactivity. Despite these promising findings, the research literature on malnutrition and externalizing behaviors remains both limited and controversial.

3.3 Lead Exposure

The toxic effects of lead on neurodevelopment are well-known, along with their consequent impact on behavior and development. Both animal studies and human studies have found that higher lead levels measured either in the bone or blood are significantly related to aggressive and delinquent behavior in childhood as well as in early adulthood. Monkeys exposed to lead show patterns of behavioral impairment resembling the deficits of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Burns et al. (1999) found that school children from a lead-smelting community had increased externalizing behavior problems. This effect remained after controlling for a number of confounding variables, including quality of the child’s home environment, maternal psychopathology, and the child’s IQ. Sciarillo et al. (1992) found that children with elevated blood lead had behavior problems scores that were 2.7 times those of the controls. In addition, even children with low lead exposure may suffer from impaired cognitive performance. For example, Needleman (1996)found that children with elevated bone lead levels but no symptoms of lead toxicity showed higher aggression and delinquency scores compared to low-lead counterparts. More recently, Wright et al. (2008) found that elevated blood lead concentrations during the prenatal and postnatal are associated with higher rates of criminal arrests in early adulthood. Nevertheless, studies in this area are to date few and limited, with many studies limited by small sample size or restriction to males only.

It is recognized that elevated blood lead concentrations are more common among children living in poverty, likely accounting for the mediating role of socioeconomic status between lead and behavioral outcomes. Environmental toxicity, micronutrients deficiency, and negative social environments may all produce adverse cognitive and behavioral outcomes, underscoring the importance of recognizing combined “health risk factors” (i.e. early health, biological, and psychosocial factors).

3.4 Birth Complications

Birth complications have been associated with antisocial behavior, particularly in the context of mitigating factors such as socioeconomic status and family stress. A large sample of Danish males demonstrated an association between birth complications and violent behavior in adulthood when combined with early maternal rejection of the child. This has been replicated in four other international samples (Sweden, Finland, Canada, and United States). Prospective longitudinal research has similarly linked obstetric complications in the context of disadvantaged familial environment to increased adult violent crime. The interaction between pregnancy complications and poor parenting also predicts to adult violence, as does the interaction between increased serious obstetric complications and family adversity.

Recent evidence suggests that cognition might represent a possible third variable accounting for the relationship between birth complications and later externalizing behavior. Liu et al. (2009) examined the influence of birth complications in predisposing to externalizing behaviors at age 11 years. Birth complications included prenatal factors such as hypertension, low blood albumin, bleeding, and other illness (e.g., diabetes, pneumonia, asthma, and epilepsy). Perinatal factors consisted of breech fetal positioning, premature rupture of membrane, fetal distress, and use of instrument delivery (e.g., forceps, Cesarean section). Postnatal factors included cyanosis and treatment with oxygen. Among a sample of nearly 1,800 children, low IQ was significantly associated with both birth complications and externalizing behavior and was found to mediate the birth complications-externalizing behavior relationship. Birth complications may directly contribute to brain dysfunction by impeding proper neurodevelopment. For example, hypoxia selectively damages the hippocampal formation, thereby impairing the limbic system, a brain area repeatedly documented by brain imaging research to be associated with aggression regulation and disinhibition.

3.5 Head Injury

The literature linking even minor head injuries to violent behavior has consistently implicated traumatic brain injury in children in increasing emotional dysregulation and behavioral problems, including externalizing behaviors. Findings from neuropsychological assessments indicate significantly greater frontal and temporal dysfunction among antisocial individuals as compared to controls. Neuropsychological impairment may in part be a function of poor maternal prenatal conditions (e.g., alcohol and tobacco exposure). Furthermore, brain imaging studies have found that aggressive prisoners exhibit significantly reduced glucose metabolism in the prefrontal cortex relative to matched controls. While findings are variable, antisocial and violent individuals as a group appear to be more likely to demonstrate neurocognitive deficits relative to non-violent controls.

3.6 Maternal Depression

Maternal depression has been consistently found to increase the potential for negative behaviors in the offspring. In one sample, maternal depression was associated with a six-fold increase in the risk for childhood externalizing behavior. More recently, a large-scale longitudinal study found that maternal depression was significantly associated with externalizing behavior boys at the age of 24 months, but by the time the children entered first grade, the relationship was stronger amongst girls. This is consistent with other researchers who have noted a moderating effect of gender on the relationship between maternal depression and externalizing behavior. The contribution of maternal mental health on childhood aggression may be mediated by family factors, such as intra-family conflict and degree of spousal support or other maternal characteristics, such as hostile and controlling behaviors.

3.7 Maternal Stress

Although not outlined in the proposed framework, maternal stress should also be recognized as an important, overarching component of health risk factors as it can result from any of the above risk factors. Prenatal maternal stress is viewed as exposure of a pregnant woman to distress - either psychosocial or environmental - which induces biochemical changes in the mother that subsequently adversely affect the fetus’ growing organ systems, including the brain. As a multifaceted factor, prenatal stress has been studied in various forms, such as chronic anxiety and psychosocial stress. Associations have been found between prenatal stress and low birth weight. Additionally, findings from animal studies have linked prenatal stress to neurodevelopmental modifications in rats. However, while recent research has demonstrated an association between prenatal stress and the developing fetus (particularly the development of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal, or HPA, axis) the link between prenatal stress and behavior remains largely unresolved.

Prenatal stress can be measured through biomarkers, such as cortisol level measurements, or through self-report, such as the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Maternal anxiety as measured by the STAI was thought to be related to a down regulation in placental 11β-HSD2, an enzyme that protects the fetus from high levels of circulating maternal cortisol. High cortisol levels are associated with adverse birth outcomes and may alter fetal development and subsequent adult health. Furthermore, prenatal stress can be denoted retrospectively. Study shows that experiencing a major negative life event was found to be associated with an alteration in the fetus’ hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis functioning. Such changes are associated with physical and mental disorders, and the study authors concluded that psychosocial stress has a strong impact on an offspring’s mental and behavioral development. Prenatal stress, including exposure to toxicants such as cocaine during pregnancy, has been shown to be linked to elevated salivary cortisol stress reactivity in school-aged children, and Susman et al. (2010) found that high cortisol reactivity in early adolescent children was directly related to antisocial and rule- breaking behavior.

3.8 Child Abuse

It is not surprising that child abuse and neglect are strongly associated with a host of negative outcomes, including behavioral disturbance in adolescence and later adulthood. Abused and neglected children were nearly five times as likely to be arrested for juvenile delinquency, with Caucasian youth demonstrating a staggering 20-fold increase in risk for juvenile arrest for violent crime. Moreover, the odds of arrest for criminal behavior as an adult were twice as high amongst victims of abuse. These trends were demonstrated in both males and females. Child maltreatment has also been linked to other negative outcomes, such as drug use and psychiatric illness, which may indirectly increase the odds for aggression and violent behavior. In a review of the long-term consequences of exposure to abuse, Teicher (2000) identified four possible pathways that may explain such findings, including abnormalities in limbic functioning (predisposing to inhibition failure and poor emotional regulation), left hemispheric damage to the hippocampus (resulting in memory impairment), decreased activation of cross-hemispheric connections, and dysregulation of emotion and attention resulting from impairment to the vernis of the cerebellum. From a psychosocial perspective, low self-esteem – a common feature of abuse victims – has been found to significantly increase the odds of aggression over time.

4. Significance of the Framework and Implications

While the proposed framework should be viewed as heuristic, it may nevertheless provide an improved conceptual framework to better understand and prevent violent and externalizing behavior in children and adolescents. Research is needed in order to test the hypotheses suggested by this framework. Nonetheless, the significance and potential contribution made by the framework can be viewed at four levels.

First, violence is a problem that exacts significant social, psychological, and economic tolls. Childhood externalizing behavior is a well-documented predisposition to adult violence. Increased awareness of contributing factors to childhood externalizing behavior can therefore help better elucidate the etiology of violence and crime. Second, the development of interventions to reduce aggression in children hinges on the ability to identify early risk factors that are amenable to change. Consequently, research on risk factors may help inform the development of such prevention programs. Additionally, because violence is considered a public health problem, prevention research plays a vital role in informing public policy and acquiring the economic support needed by local, state, and national agencies to implement violence prevention programs. From a theoretical perspective, the early health risk factor framework emphasizes interactive effects between biological and psychosocial processes, which allows for the examination of health risk factors from the main effect perspective as well as the interactive or biosocial perspective. Thus, it may help provide a clearer theoretical framework for future research on violence prediction and prevention.

Lastly, from a clinical perspective, there has been little recognition of the ways in which general medicine can contribute to violence prevention, but the framework’s emphasis on the early childhood developmental period may help facilitate greater discussion about the role of public health prevention initiatives, including those among general medicine. Health promotion programs implemented early in the child’s life, such as pre- and post-natal home visits by nurses, have been shown to play an important role in reducing future negative behaviors, including juvenile delinquency. Olds and colleagues (1998) found that children of mothers who received nurse home visits during the pre- and postnatal period displayed strikingly fewer antisocial behaviors at age 15 years than children of mothers who did not receive such visits, including 55% fewer arrests and 80% fewer criminal convictions. Nurse-visited children also had significantly lower rates of cigarette and alcohol use. Health visitation and wellness programs may be particularly useful for at-risk individuals, such as single and low-income mothers.

While there may certainly be a role for general medicine in helping to shape future child behavior, it should be noted that there are individual differences in susceptibility to early risk factors. For instance, given the same level of lead exposure in the same environment, some children do not develop high blood lead levels or behavioral problems, while others clearly do. These individual differences might reflect effects of protective or ameliorative factors, such as nutrition and parenting. More studies need to be conducted to understand why some risk factors do not uniformly result in adverse outcomes. Along these lines, one shortcoming of this proposed framework is that while interactions between the factors described earlier likely result in violent and aggressive behavior, there is a lack of empirical data to explain the underlying mechanisms of action in these interactions. Further research is needed before definitive conclusions can be made about cause-and-effect relationships and the underlying mechanisms.

Use of public prevention programs may be particularly well-suited to the early childhood developmental period. Tremblay’s research on the prevention of criminal behavior, in which he characterizes the preschool years as being “the best window of opportunity” for successfully intervening to reduce physical aggression, suggests that optimal results come with three important caveats. First, the intervention should be applied early on in the child’s life rather than waiting until adolescence. Second, it should addresses multiple risk factors rather than a single factor. Third, it should be conducted over an extended period of time. Specifically, targeting parenting behaviors, socially disruptive behaviors, and cognitive skills is advocated. Such an intervention might include teaching children verbal skills to mediate their use of physical aggression, helping young children learn to delay gratification, and teaching children cooperative skills to improve social interactions. School- and home-based programs can effectively apply such interventions or work closely with parents to impart these skills to children early on, before aggressive behavior escalates throughout the developmental trajectory.

The question of “nature versus nurture” has long been debated and continues to be controversial even today. In the past, sociologists have placed a great emphasis on social factors, while biological psychologists argued that violent behavior is rooted in pathophysiology. By focusing on the early developmental period, this framework serves as an extension of Moffitt’s LCP pathway, and the emphasis of this framework on interaction effects is consistent with the multifactorial approach (i.e., developmental, psychosocial, and environmental) described by Moffitt, Farrington, Raine, Fergusson, and others. This framework’s conceptualization is easily adaptable to prevention efforts, such as those suggested by Tremblay, as early health risk factors are amenable to intervention, as indicated by findings for Olds’ longitudinal study. By concentrating on early, modifiable risk factors, health professionals can play a greater role in secondary and tertiary prevention. This symbolizes a new and exciting interface between the contributions of scientists, educators, and practitioners alike, and may help address the longstanding public health concern of how to prevent violence.

Link to Article

Abstract

Violence and aggression are public health problems that can benefit from ongoing research into risk reduction and prevention. Current developmental theories of violence and aggression emphasize biological and psychosocial factors, particularly during adolescence. However, there has been less focus on understanding the interactive, multiplicative effects of these processes. Furthermore, little attention has been given to the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods, where prevention and intervention may yield effective results. Early health risk factors that influence negative behavioral outcomes include prenatal and postnatal nutrition, tobacco use during pregnancy, maternal depression, birth complications, traumatic brain injury, lead exposure, and child abuse. There is an ample literature to suggest that these early health risk factors may increase the likelihood of childhood externalizing behaviors, aggression, juvenile delinquency, adult criminal behavior, and/or violence. This paper proposes an early health risk factors framework for violence prediction, built on existing developmental theories of criminal behavior and supported by empirical findings. This framework addresses gaps in the adolescent psychopathology literature and presents a novel conceptualization of behavioral disturbance that emphasizes the pre-, peri-, and post-natal periods, when a child’s development is critical and the opportunity for behavioral and environmental modification is high. Implications for such a framework on violence prevention programs are discussed.

Summary

This paper explores the connection between early childhood health factors and aggressive behavior in children and adults, presenting a new framework to better understand and prevent violence.

1. Introduction

Violence is increasingly being viewed as a public health issue, and understanding the causes is crucial for prevention. While research has focused on social and biological risk factors, the role of early childhood health factors, especially those occurring during prenatal development, has been largely overlooked.

1.1 Gaps in the Current Literature

The literature on violence and aggression lacks a comprehensive approach that integrates social, biological, and health factors. Furthermore, there is a shortage of longitudinal studies examining the impact of early health risks on later behavior.

1.2 Current Theoretical Models and the Need for a New Approach

Existing models by Moffitt, Farrington, and Fergusson have significantly impacted our understanding of developmental pathways to violence but mainly focus on older children and adolescents. They also don't fully integrate biological and psychosocial factors or emphasize their interactive effects.

2. An Early Health Conceptual Framework for Violence Study

This paper proposes an early health risk factor framework that emphasizes the multiplicative effects of biological, psychosocial, and environmental factors on the development of violence. It builds on previous biosocial models of violence, incorporating early health factors as key contributors to aggressive behaviors.

2.1 Overview of the Conceptual Framework

The framework defines health risk factors as byproducts of biological and psychosocial processes during the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods. It hypothesizes that these early health risk factors, like exposure to alcohol or tobacco during pregnancy or birth complications, can increase the risk of aggressive behavior later in life.

2.2 Overview of Components of the Framework

2.2.1 Biological Risk Factors

Biological risk factors include genetic and maternal factors that can hinder fetal growth and development, such as maternal malnutrition, smoking during pregnancy, and birth complications.

2.2.2 Psychosocial Risk Factors

Psychosocial risk factors encompass experiences like child abuse, marital conflict, poor parenting, domestic violence, and community violence.

2.2.3 Protective Factors (Prevention and Intervention)

Protective factors, which can be introduced early in life, aim to reduce biological vulnerability and intervene with psychosocial influences. They include prenatal care, proper nutrition, and a healthy family environment.

2.2.4 Health Risk Factors: The Interaction between Biology and the Psychosocial Environment

This framework highlights the synergistic effects of biological and psychosocial risk factors. Both factors can interact to create a multiplicative risk for negative outcomes. For example, birth complications, influenced by both biological and social factors, can increase the likelihood of violence.

2.2.5 The Mediating Factor of the Brain

The framework recognizes the role of cognitive function as a mediating factor between risk factors and behavioral outcomes. Low IQ, particularly in areas of spatial reasoning, has been linked to aggressive behavior and may function as a mediator.

2.2.6 Impact of Early Health Risk Factors on Neurodevelopment and Subsequent Behaviors

Research suggests that certain health risk factors, such as prenatal alcohol exposure or head injuries, can impact neurodevelopment and lead to impairments in brain regions associated with emotional regulation and impulse control.

3. Empirical Evidence of Health Risk Factors during Prenatal and Early Childhood

This section provides evidence supporting the connection between various health risk factors and later aggressive behavior.

3.1 Smoking during Pregnancy

Studies have shown a strong association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and later conduct disorder and violent offending.

3.2 Malnutrition

Malnutrition during pregnancy and early childhood has been linked to behavioral problems in children. Deficiencies in macronutrients, micronutrients, and essential fatty acids may disrupt brain functioning.

3.3 Lead Exposure

Lead exposure during prenatal and childhood development has been linked to aggression and delinquency. It can impair cognitive development and affect brain regions involved in impulse control.

3.4 Birth Complications

Birth complications, particularly when combined with socioeconomic disadvantage or family stress, have been associated with an increased risk of violent behavior in adulthood.

3.5 Head Injury

Head injuries, even minor ones, can increase the risk of emotional dysregulation and behavioral problems, including aggressive behavior.

3.6 Maternal Depression

Maternal depression has been linked to an increased risk of externalizing behaviors in children. It may contribute to family conflict and negatively impact parenting behaviors.

3.7 Maternal Stress

Prenatal maternal stress can negatively affect fetal development, particularly in the brain. It may lead to altered HPA axis functioning and increase the risk of behavioral problems.

3.8 Child Abuse

Child abuse and neglect are strongly associated with a range of negative outcomes, including aggressive behavior in adolescence and adulthood. It can lead to impairments in brain regions involved in emotional regulation and impulse control.

4. Significance of the Framework and Implications

The early health risk factor framework has significant implications for understanding and preventing violence.

It highlights the importance of:

  • Identifying early risk factors: Early intervention can help prevent escalation of aggressive behaviors into more serious problems.

  • Developing prevention programs: The framework can inform the development of interventions targeting multiple risk factors.

  • Involving health professionals: Public health initiatives and healthcare professionals can play a significant role in violence prevention, particularly during early childhood.

The framework underscores the need for continued research to understand the mechanisms of action behind these interactive effects and to develop effective prevention and intervention programs.

Link to Article

Abstract

Violence and aggression are public health problems that can benefit from ongoing research into risk reduction and prevention. Current developmental theories of violence and aggression emphasize biological and psychosocial factors, particularly during adolescence. However, there has been less focus on understanding the interactive, multiplicative effects of these processes. Furthermore, little attention has been given to the pre-, peri-, and postnatal periods, where prevention and intervention may yield effective results. Early health risk factors that influence negative behavioral outcomes include prenatal and postnatal nutrition, tobacco use during pregnancy, maternal depression, birth complications, traumatic brain injury, lead exposure, and child abuse. There is an ample literature to suggest that these early health risk factors may increase the likelihood of childhood externalizing behaviors, aggression, juvenile delinquency, adult criminal behavior, and/or violence. This paper proposes an early health risk factors framework for violence prediction, built on existing developmental theories of criminal behavior and supported by empirical findings. This framework addresses gaps in the adolescent psychopathology literature and presents a novel conceptualization of behavioral disturbance that emphasizes the pre-, peri-, and post-natal periods, when a child’s development is critical and the opportunity for behavioral and environmental modification is high. Implications for such a framework on violence prevention programs are discussed.

Summary

This article is about how some things that happen before a child is born or during a child's early years can make it more likely for that child to become aggressive and violent when they grow up. The article says that these things are called "health risk factors" and they include things like smoking during pregnancy, malnutrition, lead exposure, birth complications, head injury, and child abuse. The article also says that these risk factors can work together to make it even more likely that a child will have problems with aggression. For example, a child who was born prematurely and also experiences abuse at home is more likely to be aggressive than a child who was born full-term and has a happy and supportive home.

1. Introduction

Doctors are starting to see that violence, whether it's from a kid acting out, a teen getting in trouble, or a grown-up doing something bad, is like a sickness that needs to be treated. Finding out what causes these problems before they start is really important for keeping people safe. We've been studying what makes kids and adults act out for a long time, but we still don't know enough about the things that happen to a child when they're very young that might lead to violence later.

1.1 Gaps in the Current Literature

We haven't paid enough attention to what happens before a child is born and during their first few years of life. Scientists are starting to see that the time a baby is growing in its mom's belly is a really important time for preventing future problems. We need more studies to see how these early experiences can affect a child's behavior later on.

We also need to think about how things inside a child's body and the world around them work together. It's not just about genes or how people treat a child – both things matter. Scientists haven't always looked at how these things interact to influence a child's behavior.

It's hard to study how early experiences affect a child's behavior because we need to follow the child for a long time. Scientists haven't done enough of these kinds of studies. But we've learned some things. For example, when nurses visit moms and babies during pregnancy and the first few years of life, the kids are less likely to get into trouble with the law when they grow up. These studies show that we can make a difference in a child's life by helping them early on.

1.2 Current Theoretical Models and the Need for a New Approach

Some scientists have tried to explain why kids act out by looking at how they grow and change over time. They say that some kids start having problems early in life, and these problems keep getting worse as they grow older. They say that these kids are more likely to be aggressive and get into trouble as adults. Other scientists say that some kids start having problems when they're teens, and these problems usually go away when they get older. These kids are less likely to have serious problems with violence as adults.

Scientists haven't really focused on how the things that happen to a child during their first few years of life can affect their behavior later on. They haven't really looked at how things inside a child's body and the world around them work together. This new way of thinking about violence looks at all of these things to help us understand how early experiences can lead to problems with aggression.

2. An Early Health Conceptual Framework for Violence Study

2.1 Overview of the Conceptual Framework

Scientists say that "health risk factors" are things that make it more likely for someone to get hurt, sick, or even die. These include things like being born small, smoking cigarettes, drinking alcohol, and not eating healthy. It's important to remember that these factors don't always lead to bad things, but they do increase the chances.

This new way of thinking about violence says that things that happen to a child before, during, or shortly after birth can also make it more likely that they will act out later on. These things are called "early health risk factors." Scientists think that early health risk factors can affect a child's brain and make it harder for them to control their behavior.

2.2 Overview of Components of the Framework

2.2.1 Biological Risk Factors

The things that happen inside a mom's body while a baby is growing can affect the baby's development. Things like not eating enough, smoking cigarettes, or getting sick while pregnant can hurt the baby's brain.

2.2.2 Psychosocial Risk Factors

How a child is treated can also affect their behavior. Things like being abused, having parents who fight a lot, not having good parents, or living in a dangerous neighborhood can make it more likely that a child will be aggressive.

2.2.3 Protective Factors (Prevention and Intervention)

Some things can help keep kids from becoming aggressive. These are called "protective factors". It's important to start protecting kids early on, even before they're born. This means making sure moms get good prenatal care, eat healthy, don't smoke or drink, and stay away from toxic chemicals. It's also important for parents to be loving and supportive.

2.2.4 Health Risk Factors: The Interaction between Biology and the Psychosocial Environment

We know that things inside a child's body and the world around them both play a role in how a child acts. But it's not just about adding these things together; they can actually work together to make things worse. This means that a child who has a problem with their brain (because of something that happened before they were born) might be even more likely to be aggressive if they also have problems at home.

Scientists are learning that things that happen before a child is born can change the way their genes work, which can affect their brain development.

2.2.5 The Mediating Factor of the Brain

We've learned that kids who have trouble learning and thinking are more likely to be aggressive. This is because their brains might not be working as well as they should.

Kids who have a hard time with spatial tasks (like puzzles or mazes) are especially likely to be aggressive. This might be because the right side of their brain isn't working correctly. This side of the brain helps us understand what we see and how to move our bodies. It also helps us understand emotions.

2.2.6 Impact of Early Health Risk Factors on Neurodevelopment and Subsequent Behaviors

Scientists have learned that the part of the brain that helps us control our behavior, called the prefrontal cortex, is often damaged or not working correctly in people who are aggressive. This can make it harder for them to think clearly, control their emotions, and plan for the future.

Many things can hurt a child's brain, including drinking alcohol during pregnancy, not getting enough oxygen during birth, and head injuries. Even being exposed to lead can damage the brain.

3. Empirical Evidence of Health Risk Factors during Prenatal and Early Childhood

This section talks about studies that show how the early health risk factors we talked about can affect a child's behavior later on. The article focuses on the following risk factors:

  • Smoking during pregnancy: Babies born to moms who smoke while pregnant are more likely to be aggressive and get into trouble with the law when they grow up. This is because smoking can hurt the baby's brain.

  • Malnutrition: Kids who don't get enough to eat, especially during their first few years of life, are more likely to be aggressive and have problems with behavior. This is because malnutrition can hurt the baby's brain.

  • Lead exposure: Kids who are exposed to lead, even in small amounts, are more likely to be aggressive and have problems with behavior. This is because lead is a poison that can damage the brain.

  • Birth complications: Kids who have problems during birth, such as not getting enough oxygen, are more likely to be aggressive and get into trouble with the law when they grow up. This is because birth complications can hurt the baby's brain.

  • Head injury: Kids who get head injuries, even minor ones, are more likely to have problems with behavior. This is because head injuries can hurt the brain.

  • Maternal depression: Kids who have moms who are depressed are more likely to be aggressive and have problems with behavior. This is because maternal depression can affect the child's development.

  • Maternal stress: Stress during pregnancy can also hurt the baby's brain. This can make the child more likely to be aggressive and have problems with behavior.

  • Child abuse: Kids who are abused or neglected are much more likely to be aggressive and have problems with behavior. This is because abuse can hurt the child's brain and cause emotional problems.

4. Significance of the Framework and Implications

This new way of thinking about violence is important because it helps us understand why people become aggressive. It also helps us figure out how to stop violence before it starts. We can do this by protecting kids from early health risk factors and by helping them develop good skills and healthy habits.

It's really important to help kids early on. This means starting with moms and babies during pregnancy and the first few years of life. We can help kids by teaching parents how to be good parents, by providing good healthcare, and by making sure kids get a good education.

It's not always easy to tell why some kids have problems with aggression and others don't. It might be that some kids are more sensitive to certain risk factors, or it might be that they have other things in their lives that help protect them. We need to keep studying to learn more about what makes kids vulnerable to these risk factors and how we can best help them.

Link to Article

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Liu J. (2011). Early Health Risk Factors for Violence: Conceptualization, Review of the Evidence, and Implications. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(1), 63–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2010.12.003

    Highlights