Developmental trajectories of legal socialization among serious adolescent offenders
Alex R. Piquero
Jeffrey Fagan
Edward P. Mulvey
Laurence Steinberg
Candice Odgers
SummaryOriginal

Summary

Legal socialization is the process through which individuals acquire attitudes and beliefs about the law. This is particularly important during adolescence. This study examines legal socialization among juvenile offenders over time.

2005

Developmental trajectories of legal socialization among serious adolescent offenders

Keywords legal socialization; adolescence; juvenile offenders

I. Introduction

Legal socialization is the process through which individuals acquire attitudes and beliefs about the law, legal authorities, and legal institutions. This occurs through individuals' interactions, both personal and vicarious, with police, courts, and other legal actors. To date, most of what is known about legal socialization comes from studies of individual differences among adults in their perceived legitimacy of law and legal institutions1, and in their cynicism about the law and its underlying norms.2 This work shows that adults' attitudes about the legitimacy of law are directly tied to individuals' compliance with the law and cooperation with legal authorities.3 Despite the potential importance of the development of these attitudes about law and their connection to illegal behavior, previous research on legal socialization prior to adulthood (i.e., adolescence) is rare.

Although some writers have discussed the ways in which family members and adults in the community shape children's and adolescents' attitudes and beliefs about law-related matters,4 little is known about the ways in which adolescents' legal socialization is shaped by their actual contact with the legal system. In fact, only a very small number of studies have examined legal socialization prior to adulthood.5 These studies have examined children's perceptions of law and legal procedures,6 rights and a “just world,”7 and legal reasoning.8 These early studies generally have relied either on cross-sectional or experimental designs, often with general population samples of young adults. As such, they are generally silent on the developmental component of legal socialization, the role of socializing conditions, and processes that children experience in everyday life.

The process of legal socialization should be particularly salient during adolescence, since this is the developmental period during which individuals are beginning to form an adult-like understanding of society and its institutions,9 and when they venture outside the closed systems of family and schools to experience laws and rules in a variety of social contexts where rule enforcement is more integrated with the adult world. In childhood, their experiences are limited to interactions with a small circle of authorities, such as school officials or store security guards, whose power is real, but whose formal legal status is ambiguous. More typically, whatever exposure children have had to law has been vicarious through family, friends or neighbors. But in contrast to children, adolescents' experiences with these new social and legal contexts should have more powerful influences in shaping notions of fairness and the moral underpinnings of law. Studies forecast that these notions of the fairness and morality of legal rules developed during adolescence may influence subsequent behavior in interactions with legal authorities as adults.10

Accordingly, it is reasonable to expect that interactions with legal authorities during late childhood and into adolescence should influence the development of notions of law, rules, and agreements among members of society, including adolescents, as well as the legitimacy of authority to deal fairly with citizens who violate society's rules. 11 Moreover, as a developmental outcome via socialization processes,12 legal socialization is similar to, and intertwined with, many other unfolding changes (e.g., psychosocial maturity) that occur during this period as well as with potentially powerful experiences of adolescence. One would expect perceptions about the legitimacy of law to change considerably during this time period, reflecting an ongoing dynamic between experiences and attitudes across several social contexts. In short, similar to other developmental processes which tend to grow over time and vary throughout the population, legal socialization also should exhibit growth, development, or vacillation as experience grows.

However, contact with the police and courts are infrequent among adolescents, even those in high-risk neighborhoods.13 As a result, most subjects in general population samples have little experience in the juvenile or criminal justice systems, and thus have a limited experiential basis to inform their notions regarding the law. Accordingly, studies of legal socialization in community samples of adolescents offer limited contributions to our understanding of the ways in which attitudes about the law, legal authorities, and legal institutions develop as a result of actual contact with the legal system. To better examine legal socialization as a developmental process, it is necessary to study a sample of juvenile offenders over time. In short, because adolescents are likely to vary in their patterns of legal socialization, just as they do in other developmental domains, longitudinal studies are needed to map out the natural history of development in this socio-legal domain, especially during critical developmental periods for adolescents who have nontrivial experiences with the justice system.

This study advances our understanding of legal cynicism and legitimacy in several, ways. First, we focus on adolescents. With few exceptions,14 prior studies have examined these dimensions of law-related behavior among adults. 15 If legal socialization develops during adolescence, closer measurement of this domain during that critical period is necessary to accurately identify a developmental process within the changing context of adolescence. Second, this study is the first to examine legal socialization over time in a developmental framework showing the stability or change in these domains during a critical developmental transition from late adolescence to early adulthood. Third, we examine legal socialization among active offenders. Prior work on legitimacy and legal cynicism has analyzed data from general population or community samples, where active offenders often are under-sampled. To the extent that legal cynicism and legitimacy are implicated in compliance with the law and cooperation with legal actors, we might expect these developmental outcomes to be skewed for offenders. Until this study, there has been very little research on active offenders,16 and none longitudinally, that considers the developmental patterning of legal socialization.

Accordingly, we analyze data from a juvenile court sample of adolescent offenders charged with serious crimes. Using data from four waves of interviews over eighteen months, we analyze variation in the developmental trajectories of two specific dimensions of legal socialization: legal cynicism and legitimacy. We next identify factors that might relate to the different developmental trajectories. To the best of our knowledge, the current investigation provides the first set of data on the longitudinal, within-individual patterning of two aspects of legal socialization among adolescents, specifically serious youthful offenders, a particularly important theoretical and policy-relevant group.17

II. LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

Our conception of legal socialization is rooted in larger normative views of fairness, justice, punishment, and criminal responsibility.18 These concepts are often tied to the tension between whether people obey the law because they fear punishment, or whether they comply with legal rules because compliance is a social and moral obligation, and that the law serves an essential social purpose. 19 Tyler 20 has effectively applied this conceptualization into a theory of compliance and legitimacy that contains key elements of procedural and distributive forms of justice.21 Tyler's work22 refocuses the question of whether people should obey the law to why people obey the law.23 Thus, the question of legal socialization transcends normative concerns and becomes a matter of social science and the explanation of behavior.

The legitimation of the law is the central dynamic in this socialization process. Research on legitimacy and the law is premised upon three assumptions: (1) that people have views about the legitimacy of authorities; (2) that those views shape their behavior; and (3) that those views arise out of social interactions and experiences.

Research on children and adults has identified two dimensions of legal socialization that may shape or sustain adolescent criminal behavior: institutional legitimacy and cynicism about the legal system. Institutional legitimacy refers to feelings of obligation to defer to the rules and decisions associated with legal institutions and actors.24 Tyler defines legitimacy as “the property that a rule or an authority has when others feel obligated to defer voluntarily.”25 As do others, we focus on the internalization of the responsibility to follow principles of personal morality. Legitimacy, therefore, reflects a willingness to suspend personal considerations of self-interest and to ignore personal moral values because a person thinks that an authority/rule is entitled to determine appropriate behavior within a given situation or situations.26 It is assessed by measuring the degree to which people feel that they “ought to“ obey decisions made by legal authorities, even when those decisions are viewed as wrong or not in their interests. Studies typically find that adults express strong feelings of obligation to obey the law, the police, and the courts.27

The second component of legal socialization is legal cynicism about the law and its underlying norms.28 Legal cynicism reflects general values about the legitimacy of law and social norms. It is based upon work on anomie,29 but is modified to reflect subgroup norms concerning minority urban communities.30 According to Sampson and Bartusch, “[t]he common idea is the sense in which laws or rules are not considered binding in the existential, present lives of respondents … [legal cynicism] taps variation in respondents' ratification of acting in ways that are “outside” of law and social norms.”31 Instead, respondents feel that acting in ways that are outside the law and community norms of appropriate conduct is reasonable.

These two dimensions are particularly appropriate to consider when examining legal socialization among adolescents. These notions of legitimacy and cynicism are part of a broader developmental phenomenon of self-definition with regard to authority structures common to adolescence, and the resolution of autonomy-related issues, including those involving relationships with authority figures, is a central psychosocial task of this period.32 In addition, these aspects of legal socialization are potentially influential in the development of antisocial behavior.33 In this study, we focus on the patterns of change in legal socialization among a group of serious delinquents. In order to examine individual differences in legal socialization and their relation to antisocial behavior, we need a basic understanding of how attitudes toward the legal system develop more generally.

A. THE LONGITUDINAL PATTERNING OF LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

It is reasonable to suspect that these two components of legal socialization, legitimacy and legal cynicism, should vary over time, within individuals, especially during childhood and adolescence. This developmental perspective reflects earlier research showing that the antecedents of a positive orientation toward political, legal, and social authorities play an important role in shaping adolescent and adult antisocial behavior. 34 Moreover, it seems useful to have a comprehensive understanding of the way in which these processes unfold within an individual across development. This supposition regarding the presence and importance of this developmental change is also supported by extant research in two distinct areas related to the legal socialization literature.35

The first is the literature in deterrence, specifically regarding perceived sanction threats, i.e., an individual's perception of the likelihood of being caught for committing an offense. Several studies of individual sanction threat perceptions indicate variation over time,36 and even within short time periods. For example, Minor and Harry's analysis of 488 young adults followed in a two-wave panel at two three-month intervals indicated that perceptions of sanction risk were not stable over the three month time period.37 In fact, for four of the six offenses studied (cocaine use, drunk and disorderly conduct, cheating, and shoplifting), perceptions of risk decreased significantly over the six month interval. In a second study, Paternoster and colleagues studied the issue of perceptual stability in samples of high school and college students.38 They found little perceptual stability, even within a six-month time period.39 Finally, in a two-wave panel of college students, Paternoster et al. found that as involvement in petty theft and bad check writing increased over a one-year period, perceptions of sanction certainty for both behaviors decreased.40 The authors also found that a reduction in perceived certainty was significantly related to increased involvement in both offenses.41 Finally, they found that being formally sanctioned between the two waves was related to an increase in perceived certainty.42 Much like Minor and Harry, Paternoster and colleagues also concluded that sanction threat perceptions were relatively unstable over short time periods.43

The second strand of relevant research comes from the compliance literature. Tyler's research shows that legal socialization matters in adults because it is related to compliance with law (criminality across the range of severity) and cooperation with police and other legal tasks (jury service, helping the police catch criminals).44 However, most of these are cross-sectional studies that compare people of different ages, and thus do not assess change within persons over time. With few exceptions, these cross-sectional studies have focused largely on adult samples.45

B. PRIOR RESEARCH ON LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

Existing research, although not longitudinal, nonetheless has highlighted many important aspects of the relation between legal socialization and law-abiding behavior.46 For example, in 1998 Sampson and Bartusch advanced and examined a neighborhood-level perspective on racial differences in legal cynicism, dissatisfaction with police, and the tolerance of various forms of deviance using a cross-section of 8782 residents of 343 neighborhoods in Chicago.47 Their focus was specifically on whether and how the structural characteristics of neighborhoods explained variations in legal cynicism and other attitudes.48 With respect to legal cynicism, their analyses indicated that while African Americans reported a higher level of cynicism than did whites, controlling for concentrated disadvantage eliminated this race effect.49 Importantly however, it did not alter the pattern of other important individual-level predictors of legal cynicism including gender, socioeconomic status, age, marriage, and separation/divorce.50

Using a panel design, Tyler's 1990 Chicago-based study explored the predictors of compliance with the law, specifically how recent personal experiences with the police and courts, and views of the legitimacy of the police and courts affected compliance.51 Though there are a number of important findings in his study, two particular results from the between-individual, longitudinal analyses are worth mentioning. First, legitimacy had an independent influence on compliance, even after controlling for a number of important variables, including the individual's prior level of compliance.52 Second, using contact with legal authorities as an intervening variable, Tyler found that procedural justice influenced subsequent perceptions of legitimacy even after controlling for prior perceived legitimacy suggesting that perceived procedural fairness is an important antecedent of legitimacy.53 Fagan and Tyler replicated these findings with a general population sample in New York City.54 Their work broadened the earlier analysis to include specific appraisals of the performance of the police in addition to their procedural fairness.55

Three other studies have provided relevant data. Tyler, Casper, and Fisher used a sample of 628 individuals accused of felonies interviewed prior to and following adjudication of their cases.56 Results from this study indicated that procedural justice was the key factor shaping individuals' orientations to the law and legal authorities.57 The second study pieced together six cross-sections of the General Social Survey (1972-1977) to assess stability and change in social tolerance.58 These authors found that adult levels of social tolerance were related to both pre-adult and early adult attitudinal environments.59 In a third study, Fagan and Tyler examined the contributions of legitimacy and legal cynicism to self-reported delinquency in a community sample of 212 children and adolescents ages ten to sixteen from two adjacent but racially diverse inner-city neighborhoods.60 They showed that both legitimacy and legal cynicism predicted self-reported offending in the expected directions.61 They also showed that perceptions of procedural fairness and “respect” of legal actors—police, school disciplinary staff, and store security guards—predicted both dimensions of legal socialization.62

Although these studies are certainly important for shaping the landscape of the legal socialization literature, they are limited in several respects. First, because they are largely cross-sectional, they offer little understanding of how aspects of legal socialization change over time within persons. Unfortunately, there has been no such research.63 Without longitudinal studies, it is difficult to gain a clear picture of legal socialization as a developmental process. Second, with the exception of Tyler et al., most studies have used general population samples.64 This is an important consideration in interpreting this literature because contact with legal actors is infrequent among both adults and adolescents, and thus they have little personal and/or vicarious experiential basis to inform their notions about law.65 Samples of individuals involved in the criminal justice system can provide important evidence because of their policy-relevant status.66

III. CURRENT STUDY

We analyze data from a juvenile court sample of serious adolescent offenders in two cities. Four waves of data (baseline, six, twelve, and eighteen months post-baseline) are used to model and describe variation in the developmental trajectories of these two dimensions of legal socialization. In order to address these questions, we employ an advanced statistical methodology that allows us to investigate the developmental trajectories that may characterize both legitimacy and legal cynicism, a methodology that has never been applied in the legal socialization area.

A. DATA

All subjects were participants in the Pathways to Desistance study. The ideas behind this larger investigation can be found in Mulvey et al.,67 and more methodological details of the study can be found in Schubert et al.68 The sample consists of 1355 adjudicated adolescents between the ages of fourteen and eighteen in Philadelphia (n=701) and Phoenix (n=654). The youth were selected for potential enrollment after a review of court files revealed they had been adjudicated delinquent or found guilty of a serious offense (overwhelmingly felonies). In order to ensure a sample with meaningful heterogeneity in offending activity, the proportion of juvenile males with drug offenses was limited to 15% of the sample in both cities. This restriction did not apply to females or to youths transferred to the adult system. The sample is 86% male. Twenty percent of the sample is white, 41% African-American, and 34% Hispanic. The average age at baseline was 16.04 (range 14-18), the proportion of those having at least one arrest in the past year was 50.1%, and the average number of prior arrests in the past year was almost one (range 0-9).

Informed consent was obtained from the juveniles and their parents or guardians. Eligible youths who agreed to participate in the study, and whose parents provided consent, then completed a baseline interview. For youths in the juvenile system, this interview was generally conducted within seventy-five days of their adjudication hearing. For youths in the adult system, the baseline interview was generally conducted within ninety days of the decertification hearing in Philadelphia or of the adult arraignment hearing in Phoenix (there is no waive back provision to the juvenile system under Arizona law).69 Subjects were also interviewed at six-month intervals through the eighteen-month data collection point for a total of four repeated observations. Retention at each of the four follow-up points was either 92% or 93%, especially high for a serious juvenile offender sample—particularly one followed longitudinally.

1. Variables of Interest

Legal Cynicism Following Sampson and Bartusch70 and Srole,71 our measure of legal cynicism is composed of five questions which asked respondents to rate the degree to which: (1) Laws are meant to be broken, (2) It is okay to do anything you want, (3) There are no right or wrong ways to make money, (4) If I have a fight with someone, it is no one else's business, and (5) A person has to live without thinking about the future. Response options included: (1) Strongly Disagree; (2) Somewhat Disagree; (3) Somewhat Agree; and (4) Strongly Agree. Higher values on this scale indicate higher levels of legal cynicism (range 1-4). Psychometric analyses of the scale at baseline indicated that it was reliable (alpha=.60; CFI=.99, RMSEA=.03). Legitimacy Our measure of legitimacy follows from the measure employed by Tyler, 72 Tyler and Huo, 73 and others. Specifically, respondents answered eleven questions including: (1) I have a great deal of respect for the police, (2) Overall, the police are honest, (3) I feel proud of the police, (4) I feel people should support the police, (5) The police should be allowed to hold a person suspected of a serious crime until they get enough evidence to charge them, (6) The police should be allowed to stop people on the street and require them to identify themselves, (7) The courts generally guarantee everyone a fair hearing (trial), (8) The basic rights of citizens are protected in the courts, (9) Many people convicted of crimes in the courts are actually innocent [Reverse Coded], (10) Overall, judges in the courts here are honest, and (11) Court decisions here are almost always fair. Response options included: (1) Strongly Disagree; (2) Somewhat Disagree; (3) Somewhat Agree; and (4) Strongly Agree. Again, higher values indicate higher levels of perceived legitimacy of the law (range 1-5.2). Psychometric analyses of the scale at wave one indicated that it was reliable (alpha=.80; CFI=.92, RMSEA=.07).

2. Methods

Trajectory-based models were fit to the data to examine the within-person variability across time on the legal socialization measures. A group-based approach, like the trajectory methodology, lends itself nicely to analyzing questions that are framed in terms of the shape of the developmental course of the outcome of interest.74 The trajectory methodology is based on finite mixture models, or theoretically, on the notion that more than one class of individuals underlies an observed population/distribution. Recognizing that there may be meaningful subgroups within a population that follow distinctive developmental trajectories, Nagin and Land developed a modeling strategy that makes no parametric assumptions with respect to the distribution of persistent unobserved heterogeneity in the population.75 Unlike other techniques, the semi-parametric mixed (SPM) poisson model assumes that the distribution of unobserved persistent heterogeneity is discrete rather than continuous, and thus the mixing distribution is viewed as multinomial (i.e., a categorical variable). Each category within the multinomial mixture can be viewed as a point of support, or grouping, for the distribution of individual heterogeneity. The model, then, estimates a separate point of support (or grouping) for as many distinct groups as can be identified in the data.76 In other words, the trajectory methodology takes individuals who resemble one another on the outcome of interest and assigns them to a particular group. Individuals in each respective group resemble one another more so than they do the individuals assigned to other groups. The groups, then, are mutually exclusive such that each individual can only be a member of one trajectory group.

It is important to remember that the trajectory groups approximate population differences in developmental trajectories. A higher number of points of support (groups) yields a discrete distribution that more closely approximates what may be a true continuous distribution.77 Further, because each individual has a non-zero probability of belonging to each of the various groups identified, s/he is assigned to the group to which s/he has the highest probability of belonging. This is a particularly important feature of this methodology because it allows researchers to assess the claims of extant developmental models that make predictions about different groups of offenders. This cannot be accomplished with approaches that treat unobserved heterogeneity in a continuous fashion. In short, the trajectory methodology is well-suited for research problems with a taxonomic dimension, the purpose of which is to chart distinctive developmental trajectories, and to understand what factors account for their distinctiveness.78

Because we are dealing with continuous psychometric, yet bounded data, the Censored Normal version of the SPM is appropriate here. To evaluate model fit we follow extant research and use the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC). BIC, or the log-likelihood evaluated at the maximum likelihood estimate less one-half the number of parameters in the model times the log of the sample size,79 tends to favor more parsimonious models than likelihood ratio tests when used for model selection. Following previous research,80 we use an iterative procedure for identifying meaningful groups. The approach we take is to begin with a one-group model and continue along the modeling space to two, three, four, five, and six groups, until we maximize the BIC.81

The approach taken here is primarily descriptive. We are concerned with whether there are meaningful subgroups of adolescents showing different patterns of legal socialization, what the legal socialization trajectories look like, and what sorts of demographic variables distinguish individuals showing these different trajectories.82 It is important to note that because the groups are intended as an approximation of a more complex underlying reality, the objective is not to identify the “true” number of groups. Instead, the aim is to identify as simple a model as possible that displays the distinctive features of the population distribution of trajectories.83 All trajectory models were fit using the Proc Traj procedure in SAS.

Descriptive statistics for the legal socialization variables and correlations among those variables over time may be found in Tables ​Tables 1 and ​and 2, respectively. As shown in Table 1, with regard to legal cynicism, the average value and the variation around that average (i.e., standard deviation) does not change much over time; however, there is some more change about both the mean and variation in legitimacy over time.

Screenshot 2024-09-08 at 22.59.17Screenshot 2024-09-08 at 23.01.04

B. RESULTS

1. Legal Cynicism Trajectories: The best fitting model (i.e., maximization of the BIC statistic) for the legal cynicism data was a four-group model. Figure 1 shows the trajectories of expected (i.e., predicted on the basis of estimated model parameters) cynicism scores for each latent class implied by the model.

Screenshot 2024-09-08 at 23.03.24

Two findings immediately stand out in Figure 1. First, there are mean-level differences at the intercept (baseline) for all four groups. For example, at baseline, group 1 averages 1.40 (less cynical) on the cynicism scale while group 4 averages close to 3.0 (more cynical). Second, with the exception of group 4 (which includes 4.2% of the sample), groups 1 (17.9% of the sample), 2 (43.8% of the sample), and 3 (34.1% of the sample) exhibit flat and stable levels of cynicism across the eighteen-month time period. Group 4 exhibits some increasing cynicism between T1 and T4. In short, for the majority of the study participants, there is very little change in perceptions of legal cynicism over the time frame captured here.

For each study participant (and subsequent latent class) we computed the maximum posterior membership probabilities, the results of which may be found in Table 3. Specifically, we follow the model's ability to sort individuals into the trajectory group to which they have the highest probability of belonging (the ‘maximum probability’ procedure). Based on the model coefficient estimates, the probability of observing each individual's longitudinal pattern of legal cynicism is computed conditional on their being, respectively, in each of the latent classes. The individual is then assigned to the group to which they have the highest probability of belonging. This procedure, of course, does not guarantee perfect assignment, but higher posterior probabilities are indicative of more acceptable levels of class assignment.

Screenshot 2024-09-08 at 23.05.06

Here, the mean assignment probabilities for each group are all quite high (all above .75) suggesting that the majority of individuals can be classified to a particular latent trajectory with high probability. For example, for group 1, the mean posterior probability was .825; for group 2, it was .783; for group 3, it was .792; and for group 4, it was .800.

2. What Factors Relate to Legal Cynicism? In Table 4, we examined how the groups varied along a series of variables, several of which have been examined in the adult literature.84 We looked at: age (scored continuously, range 14-18), age-specific dummy variables (age 14 through age 18); site (1=Philadelphia, 2=Phoenix), the number of prior arrests in the past year (scored continuously, range 0-9), the age at first arrest (scored continuously, range 9-18), race-specific dummy variables (white, Hispanic, and African-American); gender (1=male, 2=female), whether the participant was locked-up at the baseline interview (1=yes, 0=no), and an adapted version of the Procedural Justice Inventory85 that focused on the adolescent's perception of fairness and equity connected with police and court processing.

Variables

Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

Group 4

F-Value

Site

1.446

1.468

1.530

1.369

2.751*

Priors past yr.

0.78

0.88

1.12

0.91

4.784*

Age at first

14.485

14.514

14.384

14.152

1.097

Black

0.459

0.403

0.405

0.434

0.843

White

0.255

0.236

0.137

0.130

7.520*

Hispanic

0.246

0.312

0.401

0.413

6.758*

Gender

1.22

1.16

1.08

1.02

12.602*

Locked-up

0.327

0.302

0.388

0.326

2.957*

Age

15.978

16.049

16.055

16.173

0.476

Age 14

0.148

0.115

0.111

0.108

0.790

Age 15

0.187

0.187

0.195

0.130

0.386

Age 16

0.293

0.314

0.296

0.304

0.183

Age 17

0.276

0.299

0.319

0.391

1.024

Age 18

0.093

0.083

0.077

0.065

0.249

PJ-Police (Overall)

2.867

2.762

2.732

2.641

4.632*

PJ-Court (Overall)

3.325

3.322

3.209

3.108

3.357*

PJ-Police (Direct)

2.894

2.807

2.785

2.705

2.569*

PJ-Police (Indirect)

2.782

2.632

2.577

2.452

5.736*

PJ-Court (Direct)

3.430

3.291

3.296

3.140

3.452*

PJ-Court (Indirect)

3.365

3.254

3.221

3.142

2.957*

Conceptually, procedural justice taps the experiential and affective basis for translating interactions with legal processes into perceptions and evaluations of the law and the legal actors who enforce it. Fair treatment allows people to attribute legitimacy to authorities and creates a set of obligations to conform to their norms. It communicates to participants directly, and vicariously to people in contact with other participants in legal interactions, that laws are both legitimate and moral. Fair treatment also may reduce feelings of anger that lead to rule breaking.86 It strengthens ties to the law, a pivotal antecedent of delinquency,87 while at the same time counteracting labeling processes that are marginalizing and stigmatizing.88 Tyler and several other studies report that fair treatment is positively related to law abiding behavior among both younger and older adults.89 In developmental terms, fair treatment strengthens ties and attachments to the laws and social norms, as well as group membership. Such procedural justice judgments are found to both shape reactions to personal experiences with legal authorities90 and to be important in assessments based upon the general activities of the police.91

Following the operationalizations found in prior research, the measures are designed to tap several dimensions of fair treatment: correctability, ethicality, representativeness, and consistency.92 The outcomes of this process include evaluations of law and its underlying norms: legitimacy and legal cynicism.93 Following prior research,94 we hypothesize that males, minorities, older adolescents, those with prior arrests and an earlier age at first arrest, those who were locked-up, and those who perceive lower procedural justice regarding the police and courts should hold more cynical attitudes.

The results from the ANOVA analyses indicate that the four groups differ across twelve variables: site, priors, white, Hispanic, gender, lock-up status, and all six procedural justice variables. Regarding site, there are slightly more Philadelphia participants in group 4 (high cynicism group), whereas there are more Phoenix participants especially in group three. As would be expected, those individuals in the two groups (groups 3 and 4) who have the highest cynicism values also have the largest number of prior arrests. Group 1, who had the lowest cynicism scores, had the lowest number of priors in the past year. There were more white participants in the two lowest cynicism groups (1 and 2), while there were more Hispanic participants in the two groups (groups 3 and 4) with the highest cynicism values over the eighteen-month period. Age was not significantly different across the four cynicism trajectories. Also, there were more females in the lowest legal cynicism groups (1 and 2) than in the higher cynicism groups. Finally, the groups differ along all six procedural justice dimensions at baseline. Across almost all comparisons shown in Table 4, the two lowest cynicism groups consistently report higher (i.e., more favorable) procedural justice perceptions, while the two highest cynicism groups consistently report lower (i.e., less favorable) procedural justice perceptions—and this is the case across all domains and for the general and specific scales.

The high level of stability over time in legal cynicism and diversity in levels over time suggests that inter-individual differences among study participants in their cynicism about the legal system likely were established before their first assessment in this study, perhaps as young as fifteen years of age. In other words, the groups derived from the trajectory analyses may indicate statistically meaningful cut-points for dividing the group on a continuous measure of cynicism that is stable across the different time points.

Next, we examine what baseline case characteristics are associated with the average level of cynicism over the four interviews, using an ordinary least squares regression approach that considers all the variables in a single step. The outcome variable, the average value of legal cynicism across the four waves, was obtained by summing the four cynicism scores and then dividing by four (M=2.03, SD=0.48). We used the same variables for this analysis as we did for the ANOVA's presented above.95 These results are presented in Table 5.

Table 5 OLS Regression Predicting Average Legal Cynicism

Variable

B

SE(B)

Age 15

0.042

0.051

Age 16

0.056

0.048

Age 17

0.078

0.050+

Age 18

−0.029

0.067

Site

0.025

0.037

Priors

0.026

0.011*

Age first

−0.007

0.010

Black

0.052

0.042

Hispanic

0.161

0.037*

Gender

−0.167

0.041*

Locked-up

0.010

0.031

PJ-Police-Overall

−0.046

0.032

PJ-Court-Overall

−0.068

0.031*

Constant

2.484

0.175*

R-Square

.054

*p<.05 (2 tailed-test),+p<.10

Six variables attain significance in this model estimation. First, seventeen-year-old participants were more likely to have higher cynicism than their age-fourteen counterparts, though this result was only marginally significant. None of the other age groups differed from the age-fourteen participants. Additionally, individuals with a higher number of prior arrests in the past year were more likely to report a higher average cynicism across the eighteen-month follow-up period. Third, Hispanic juveniles were more likely than white juveniles to report higher cynicism. Fourth, females were less likely than males to have higher cynicism scores. Finally, with regard to the procedural justice perceptions, Table 5 shows that higher scores on the measure of procedural justice regarding the court are associated with lower average legal cynicism scores. In contrast, individuals' score on the procedural justice scale with regard to police practices does not significantly predict their score on the measure of legal cynicism.96

3. Legitimacy Trajectories: Next, we turned to a trajectory-based analysis of individuals' perceptions of the legitimacy of law over the eighteen-month follow-up period. A five-trajectory solution was the best fitting model, using the BIC decision rule. The plot for this solution is shown in Figure 2.

Figure 2: Predicted Legitimacy Trajectories

Screenshot 2024-09-09 at 20.20.24

Several findings are of interest in the legitimacy trajectory analysis. First, there are mean-level differences at the intercept (baseline) for all five groups. For example, at baseline, group 1 averages 1.50 (low legitimacy) while group 4 averages close to 3.0 (more legitimacy). Second, over 40% of the sample, groups 1 (10.1% of the sample) and 2 (32.6% of the sample), have very flat and stable trajectories over time. Groups 3 (42.2% of the sample) and 4 (13.4% of the sample), which are reasonably stable over the follow-up period, start at different baseline levels but appear to be converging by the eighteen-month period, with group 4 coming to perceive less legitimacy over time but group 3 perceiving somewhat more legitimacy over time. Third, the only group in which there is a dramatic increase in perceptions of legitimacy over time is very small—group 5 which accounts for only 1.7% of the sample. These individuals start out with relatively high legitimacy perceptions (close to 3.0) but increase to around 4.0 by the eighteen-month interview. Although this group is certainly of interest, it is important to note the very small number of individuals in this group. In short, with a few exceptions, for the majority of the study participants, there is very little change in perceptions of legitimacy across the eighteen-month period.

Again, we computed the maximum posterior membership probabilities for membership in the five legitimacy groups (see Table 6). Here, the mean assignment probabilities for each group are all quite high (i.e., all above .75) suggesting that the majority of individuals can be classified to a particular latent trajectory group with high probability. For group 1, the mean posterior probability was .819; for group 2, it was .774; for group 3, it was .802; for group 4, it was .815; and for group 5 it was .879.

Screenshot 2024-09-09 at 20.23.02

4. What Factors Relate to Legitimacy? In order to examine how the five groups vary along a set of baseline characteristics, we present a series of analysis-of-variance estimates. For the legitimacy analysis, we use the same variables employed in the prior analysis of scores on the measure of legal cynicism. These results may be found in Table 7.

Table 7: Analysis of Variance—Legitimacy Means Presented By Group

Variables

Group 1

Group 2

Group 3

Group 4

Group 5

F- Value

Site

1.201

1.382

1.538

1.695

1.900

28.479*

Priors past yr.

1.04

1.02

0.96

0.69

0.55

2.619*

Age at first

14.491

14.453

14.434

14.396

15.200

1.109

Black

0.631

0.517

0.370

0.189

0.050

24.585*

White

0.105

0.160

0.224

0.286

0.350

5.918*

Hispanic

0.219

0.284

0.357

0.445

0.550

6.724*

Gender

1.09

1.10

1.15

1.19

1.40

6.498*

Locked-up

0.403

0.377

0.324

0.250

0.150

3.703*

Age

16.368

16.455

16.003

15.652

16.100

8.604*

Age 14

0.087

0.084

0.130

0.213

0.000

5.945*

Age 15

0.114

0.173

0.197

0.231

0.300

2.202

Age 16

0.315

0.333

0.283

0.286

0.350

0.889

Age 17

0.307

0.320

0.316

0.225

0.300

1.430

Age 18

0.175

0.088

0.072

0.042

0.050

4.477*

PJ-Police (Overall)

2.399

2.579

2.849

3.189

3.090

77.233*

PJ-Court (Overall)

2.713

3.093

3.306

3.662

3.569

76.795*

PJ-Police (Direct)

2.439

2.602

2.897

3.285

3.143

75.574*

PJ-Police (Indirect)

2.281

2.512

2.707

2.900

2.920

20.574*

PJ-Court (Direct)

2.731

3.156

3.381

3.834

3.733

60.770*

PJ-Court (Indirect)

2.678

3.092

3.364

3.684

3.599

58.213*

The results from the ANOVA analysis indicate that the five groups differ across sixteen variables: age (continuous), site, priors, African-American, white, Hispanic, locked-up, age-fourteen, and age-eighteen, gender, and all six procedural justice measures. As the table indicates, older individuals tend to hold lower perceptions of legitimacy while younger individuals are more likely found in group 4 who have relatively high perceptions of legitimacy. Regarding site, there are slightly more Phoenix participants in group 4 and 5, the two groups with the highest legitimacy scores. The two groups with the lowest legitimacy scores over time also have the highest number of prior arrests, while individuals in group 5 (the high legitimacy group) have the lowest number of prior arrests. Regarding race, while there are more African-Americans in groups 1 and 2 (the groups with the lowest legitimacy scores), there are more whites and Hispanics in the two groups (4 and 5) with the highest legitimacy values. The two low legitimacy groups (1 and 2) also had the highest values of lock-up, while the two high legitimacy groups (4 and 5) had the lowest values of lock-up. Two age coefficients were significantly different across the five groups: age-fourteen and age-eighteen: whereas there were a higher proportion of fourteen-year-olds in groups 3 and 4 (high but stable legitimacy scores), there were a higher number of eighteen-year olds in groups 1,2, and 3 having the lowest legitimacy scores. Females were more likely to be in the higher legitimacy group than they were to be in the lower legitimacy group. Finally, the legitimacy groups differed along all six procedural justice dimensions. Across these comparisons, the two groups with the lowest legitimacy (1 and 2) consistently scored lowest on all six procedural justice markers, whereas the two groups with the highest legitimacy (4 and 5) consistently scored higher on all six procedural justice markers.

As was the case for legal cynicism, there was great stability in perceptions of legitimacy over the eighteen-month period. Because of this, we investigated how the baseline characteristics predicted average legitimacy scores. This outcome variable, the average value of legitimacy, was obtained by summing the four cynicism scores and then dividing by four (M=2.38, SD=0.49). These results can be found in Table 8.

Table 8: OLS Regression Predicting Average Legitimacy

Variable

B

SE(B)

Age 15

−0.058

0.044

Age 16

−0.090

0.042*

Age 17

−0.078

0.044+

Age 18

−0.098

0.058+

Site

0.115

0.032*

Priors

−0.012

0.010

Age first

−0.001

0.008

Black

−0.125

0.036*

Hispanic

−0.015

0.032

Gender

0.082

0.035*

Locked-up

−0.082

0.027*

PJ-Police-Overall

0.272

0.028*

PJ-Court-Overall

0.223

0.027*

Constant

0.829

0.152*

R-Square

0.333

*p<.05 (2 tailed-test),

+p<.10 As can be seen, a number of variables attain significance in this model estimation. First, while all four age effects are negative indicating that compared to fourteen-year-olds, older individuals are less likely to hold high legitimacy perceptions, only the coefficient for sixteen-year-olds attains significance at p<.05, whereas the age-seventeen and age-eighteen groups are marginally significantly different from the fourteen-year-olds. Second, Phoenix juveniles are more likely to have higher perceptions of legitimacy than Philadelphia juveniles. Third, compared to whites, African-Americans report lower legitimacy perceptions. Fourth, individuals who were locked-up were more likely to have lower perceptions of legitimacy. Fifth, females are more likely than males to have higher legitimacy perceptions. Finally, measures of procedural justice with respect to both police and court procedures were significant in predicting average legitimacy scores. Individuals with higher procedural justice perceptions regarding both the police and the courts were more likely to hold higher average legitimacy scores.97

5. Concordance Between Legal Cynicism and Legitimacy Trajectories

It is also of interest to examine the common ground of group membership for both legal cynicism and legitimacy. It is expected that individuals who hold highly cynical attitudes towards the law will be less likely to afford legitimacy to the law and legal procedures.98 Table 9 presents a cross-tabulation of the four-group legal cynicism model and the five-group legitimacy model. Several findings emerge in this simple cross-tabulation. First, we confirm the negative relationship between legal cynicism and legitimacy (described earlier in Table 2).99 The two variables are significantly related to one another (χ = 167.605, p<.001) and modestly associated (Φ = .352). Second, individuals with the lowest reported cynicism (groups 1 and 2) also report high legitimacy perceptions. In fact, of the twenty individuals who reported the highest perceptions of legitimacy (group 5), 19 were in one of the two lowest legal cynicism groups, 1 (12) and 2 (7). Third, of the forty-six individuals in the highest cynicism group (4), forty-one of them, or 89%, are found in the two lowest legitimacy groups (16 are in group 1 and 25 in group 2), confirming the expectation raised above.

Table 9: Cross-Tabulation of 4-Group Legal Cynicism and 5-Group Legitimacy Trajectories. Observed values shown first, expected values in parentheses.

Screenshot 2024-09-09 at 21.22.16

6. Alternative Stratified Trajectory Specifications: In a series of supplemental analyses, we also estimated trajectories of legal cynicism and legitimacy by stratifying the sample as to whether they had any priors in the year before the baseline interview (no/yes), by lock-up status at baseline (no/yes), and then for each of the age groups between fourteen and eighteen. In each of these analyses, the results indicated that individuals' perceptions of the legitimacy of law as well as their degree of cynicism about the legal system were stable over time. That is, although individuals with priors reported different baseline levels of legitimacy and legal cynicism compared to first-time offenders, both groups' perceptions were highly stable over time. The same finding emerged for individuals who were and were not locked-up. Finally, the stratified age analyses also revealed stability within each age group (fourteen through eighteen) in both legal cynicism and legitimacy, but different intercepts at baseline.100

C. DISCUSSION

This was the first study of the longitudinal, within-individual trajectories of two fundamental aspects of legal socialization—legal cynicism and legitimacy of law—among a large sample of serious juvenile offenders. Three main findings emerged from our effort.

The first and most important is the strong stability of both legal cynicism and legitimacy over the eighteen-month period after court disposition among this group of serious adolescent offenders. With few exceptions, there was little developmental change in these dimensions of legal socialization over time, and this was the case both overall as well as within subgroups defined by the number of priors, lock-up status, and age. At the same time, while there does not appear to be a significant amount of systematic change in these variables, there is some oscillation (i.e., movement away) over the eighteen-month period away from the mean.

Second, although individuals' cynicism about the legal system and perceptions of the legitimacy of law were highly stable over time, they do vary in mean levels of legitimacy and legal cynicism, and these mean differences were consistently related to other factors. For example, individuals with more priors reported greater cynicism than individuals with fewer priors, and Hispanics reported more cynicism than whites. On the other hand, when we examined the determinants of legitimacy, we found strong age effects, such that older individuals were less likely to perceive the law as legitimate than were fourteen-year-olds. In light of the fact that very few individuals in the sample as a whole evinced change in perceptions of legitimacy over time, this may indicate that perceptions of the law's legitimacy change very little after age fourteen, perhaps because these more general and less situational sorts of attitudes and beliefs have become strongly entrenched. Additionally, juveniles who were locked-up, as well as African-Americans, were more likely to have lower legitimacy perceptions compared to juveniles not locked-up and whites. Finally, we also found consistent evidence regarding the relationship between the more situationally-based procedural justice perceptions and both measures of legal socialization. Across all comparisons, individuals with high procedural justice perceptions regarding the police and the courts tended to have lower legal cynicism and higher legitimacy, thereby suggesting that situational experiences with criminal justice personnel influence more general attitudes about the law and legal system.

Third, when we cross-tabulated the trajectories of legal cynicism and legitimacy we found that individuals who reported the lowest legal cynicism also reported the highest legitimacy, while individuals reporting the highest cynicism were highly likely to also report lower legitimacy. In conjunction, these two faces of legal socialization provide detailed information about the experience-based normative orientation of adolescents with respect to the law and legal actors.

Although the results are the first longitudinal analyses in the legal socialization literature, our data are limited in some respects. First, the trajectory methodology itself has some shortcomings. Because it assumes that unobserved heterogeneity is drawn from a discrete (multinomial) probability distribution, there may be model misspecification bias if unobserved individual differences are actually drawn from a continuous distribution. Also, classification of individuals to distinctive groups will never be perfect,101 a finding that is somewhat mitigated by the relatively strong posterior probability assignments observed herein, and the confirmatory substantive results observed in the cross-tabulation analysis. Still, care should be taken on the reification of distinct groups of legal cynicism and/or legitimacy.

Second, our data assessed perceptions of legal socialization over an eighteen-month time period. It may be that this is not long enough to observe change. We believe that it is a sufficiently long period, however, because (a) the deterrence literature indicates that perceptions change even over three month time periods and (b) as a serious offender sample, participants in this study are likely to have many experiences of criminal justice contact, either through personal or vicarious channels,102 which should increase the likelihood of finding change in attitudes and beliefs. It may be that the ingredients that beliefs, attitudes, and legal socialization are comprised of, is more enduring and less likely to be impacted by experiences with the criminal justice system than the ‘perceptions’ and the objective ‘knowledge’ of the criminal justice system that may be affected by sanctions. This may be an important distinction to highlight in future research because of its potential to inform us of the types of impact the criminal justice system has and does not have on offenders.

Third, our findings of stability in legal socialization may be due to the fact that we captured our sample during an age range where there simply is not much change. That is, much of the instability of perceptions may occur earlier in life (i.e., before fourteen) or in adulthood (i.e., after twenty-one). Alternatively, the finding of stability may reflect some sort of selection effect in that these adolescents have experienced variability in cynicism and legitimacy but after numerous contacts with the criminal justice system their levels of legal socialization become established (i.e., stable). Recent work by Fagan and Tyler suggests that this indeed is the case.103 Using a community sample from two New York City neighborhoods, they showed that both legitimacy and legal cynicism changed over time from ages ten through fourteen, and then became more stable by age sixteen. Legal cynicism and legitimacy were well predicted by the quality of respondents' experiences with the police, but not necessarily by the extent of contact, either direct or vicarious through peers and family members. This suggests that the stability in this adolescent offender sample may not be an artifact of selection of persons with more frequent and intense contacts with legal authorities, but instead may be a robust finding that applies both to offenders and non-offenders.

Fourth, because our effort was primarily descriptive, we did not document the pre- and post-adolescent characteristics that are associated with changes in legal socialization and how these changes influence criminal activity and compliance into adulthood. It is of great interest and import to examine whether, and which specific types of, external events/experiences influence legal socialization trajectories over time.104 Finally, it was beyond the scope of the current study to examine the source/origin of perceptions of legitimacy and legal cynicism. Since parents tend to be one of—if not the primary—socializing agent in adolescents' lives influencing their moral values, and religious and political beliefs,105 it would be particularly useful in subsequent research to examine the concordance of parent-child perceptions of legal socialization to determine the extent to which parents and children share similar legal socialization perceptions.

These limitations notwithstanding, our effort provides the first depiction of how two distinct legal socialization constructs, legal cynicism and legitimacy, unfold over time in a sample of serious juvenile offenders. Based on the evidence in the deterrence/compliance literatures, we had suspected that we would find evidence to suggest that such perceptions would be more dynamic than the static results observed. Theories of justice generally, and procedural justice/legal socialization in particular, have not said much about the longitudinal patterning of legal socialization during adolescence. Our results indicate that some heavy theoretical lifting is in order to understand why such perceptions are highly stable. Granted, this may be due to the fact that we used an offender-based sample, but we would be very surprised if this was the driving force behind the stability of such perceptions. It is critical that other researchers collect the requisite data to determine the replicability of our results.

Summary

This study examines the longitudinal development of legal cynicism and legitimacy in a sample of serious juvenile offenders. The research aims to address the lack of longitudinal studies on legal socialization among adolescents, particularly those with significant experience in the justice system.

I. Introduction

Legal socialization is the process through which individuals develop attitudes and beliefs about the law and legal institutions. Previous research has primarily focused on adult populations, leaving a gap in our understanding of legal socialization during adolescence.

This study addresses this gap by focusing on adolescents, specifically serious juvenile offenders. The research employs a longitudinal design, collecting data from four waves of interviews over eighteen months. The study's objectives are to:

  • Examine the developmental trajectories of legal cynicism and legitimacy among adolescent offenders.

  • Identify factors that may contribute to the observed trajectories.

II. LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

The study conceptualizes legal socialization within the broader framework of normative views of fairness, justice, and punishment. Two key dimensions of legal socialization are identified:

  • Institutional Legitimacy: This refers to individuals' feelings of obligation to defer to the rules and decisions of legal authorities.

  • Legal Cynicism: This reflects generalized attitudes about the legitimacy of law and social norms, characterized by a sense that laws are not binding and that acting outside the law is justifiable.

A. THE LONGITUDINAL PATTERNING OF LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

The study posits that both legitimacy and legal cynicism should vary over time, particularly during adolescence. This assumption is supported by previous research in deterrence and compliance literatures, which highlights the dynamic nature of individuals' perceptions of sanction threats and their compliance with the law.

B. PRIOR RESEARCH ON LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

Prior research, largely cross-sectional, has examined the relationship between legal socialization and law-abiding behavior. Studies have highlighted the influence of neighborhood characteristics, personal experiences with legal authorities, and perceptions of procedural justice on individuals' legal socialization. However, these studies are limited by their cross-sectional design and reliance on general population samples, which may not adequately represent the experiences of juvenile offenders.

III. CURRENT STUDY

This study utilizes data from the Pathways to Desistance study, a longitudinal investigation of serious juvenile offenders. The study's sample consists of 1355 adolescents between the ages of 14 and 18 who have been adjudicated delinquent or found guilty of serious offenses.

1. Variables of Interest

  • Legal Cynicism: This is measured using a five-item scale that assesses respondents' agreement with statements such as "Laws are meant to be broken" and "There are no right or wrong ways to make money." Higher scores indicate greater legal cynicism.

  • Legitimacy: This is measured using an eleven-item scale that assesses respondents' agreement with statements such as "I have a great deal of respect for the police" and "The courts generally guarantee everyone a fair hearing." Higher scores indicate greater perceived legitimacy of the legal system.

2. Methods

Trajectory-based models, specifically semi-parametric mixed (SPM) Poisson models, are used to analyze the longitudinal data. The trajectory methodology aims to identify subgroups of individuals who exhibit distinct patterns of change over time in their legal cynicism and legitimacy.

B. RESULTS

1. Legal Cynicism Trajectories

A four-group model was identified as the best-fitting model for legal cynicism.

  • Group 1 (17.9% of the sample): Low cynicism, stable over time.

  • Group 2 (43.8% of the sample): Moderate cynicism, stable over time.

  • Group 3 (34.1% of the sample): Moderate cynicism, stable over time.

  • Group 4 (4.2% of the sample): High cynicism, exhibiting slight increase over time.

2. What Factors Relate to Legal Cynicism?

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated that the four cynicism groups differed significantly across several variables:

  • Site: More Philadelphia participants in group 4 (high cynicism).

  • Priors: Individuals with more prior arrests in the past year were more likely to be in groups with higher cynicism.

  • Race: More Hispanic participants in the higher cynicism groups, while more white participants in the lower cynicism groups.

  • Gender: More females in the lower cynicism groups.

  • Lock-up Status: More individuals who were locked-up at baseline in the higher cynicism groups.

  • Procedural Justice: Individuals in the lower cynicism groups consistently reported higher perceptions of procedural justice.

An OLS regression model further confirmed the relationship between prior arrests, race, gender, and procedural justice perceptions and average legal cynicism scores.

3. Legitimacy Trajectories

A five-group model was identified as the best-fitting model for legitimacy.

  • Group 1 (10.1% of the sample): Low legitimacy, stable over time.

  • Group 2 (32.6% of the sample): Moderate legitimacy, stable over time.

  • Group 3 (42.2% of the sample): Moderate legitimacy, increasing slightly over time.

  • Group 4 (13.4% of the sample): High legitimacy, decreasing slightly over time.

  • Group 5 (1.7% of the sample): Very high legitimacy, increasing dramatically over time.

4. What Factors Relate to Legitimacy?

ANOVA indicated that the five legitimacy groups differed significantly across several variables:

  • Age: Older individuals tended to have lower legitimacy perceptions, while younger individuals were more likely to be in groups with higher legitimacy.

  • Site: More Phoenix participants in the higher legitimacy groups.

  • Priors: Individuals with more prior arrests were more likely to be in groups with lower legitimacy.

  • Race: More African-American participants in the lower legitimacy groups, while more white and Hispanic participants in the higher legitimacy groups.

  • Lock-up Status: More individuals who were locked-up at baseline were in the lower legitimacy groups.

  • Procedural Justice: Individuals in the lower legitimacy groups consistently reported lower perceptions of procedural justice.

An OLS regression model confirmed the relationship between age, site, race, lock-up status, and procedural justice perceptions with average legitimacy scores.

5. Concordance Between Legal Cynicism and Legitimacy Trajectories

A cross-tabulation of the legal cynicism and legitimacy groups showed a strong negative association between the two constructs. Individuals with lower legal cynicism tended to have higher legitimacy, while those with higher cynicism tended to have lower legitimacy.

6. Alternative Stratified Trajectory Specifications

Supplemental analyses, stratified by prior offenses, lock-up status, and age, consistently revealed stable patterns of legal cynicism and legitimacy over time, suggesting that these constructs are relatively resistant to change during adolescence, even among serious offenders.

C. DISCUSSION

The study's findings provide important insights into the longitudinal patterning of legal socialization among serious juvenile offenders. Key findings include:

  • Stability of Legal Socialization: Legal cynicism and legitimacy exhibited strong stability over the eighteen-month period, indicating that these constructs are relatively fixed during adolescence.

  • Mean Differences in Legal Socialization: While change was minimal, the study observed significant mean differences in legal cynicism and legitimacy associated with factors such as prior arrests, race, gender, site, lock-up status, and perceptions of procedural justice.

  • Negative Association between Cynicism and Legitimacy: A strong negative relationship was found between legal cynicism and legitimacy, further highlighting the interconnected nature of these constructs.

The study's limitations include the use of a trajectory methodology that relies on a discrete distribution of heterogeneity, the relatively short follow-up period, the focus on a sample of offenders, and the lack of exploration of pre- and post-adolescent characteristics.

Despite these limitations, the study provides valuable insights into the dynamics of legal socialization among serious juvenile offenders. The findings suggest that legal cynicism and legitimacy are relatively stable constructs during adolescence, but that their mean levels are influenced by a variety of factors. Future research should address the study's limitations and further explore the interplay between legal socialization, criminal activity, and compliance into adulthood.

Summary

This study investigates the developmental trajectories of legal cynicism and legitimacy among serious juvenile offenders. Using longitudinal data from four waves of interviews, the study examines how these attitudes change over time within individuals during late adolescence and early adulthood. The research also identifies factors that might influence these developmental trajectories.

I. Introduction

Legal socialization is the process through which individuals develop their beliefs and attitudes towards the law, legal institutions, and authorities. Past research has primarily focused on adult perceptions of legal legitimacy and cynicism, showing a strong connection between these attitudes and an individual's compliance with the law. However, little is known about the development of these attitudes in adolescence, a crucial period when individuals are forming their understanding of society and its institutions.

This study addresses this gap in the literature by focusing on adolescents, specifically serious youthful offenders. It is the first to examine legal socialization longitudinally among this group, providing insights into the stability or change in attitudes during a critical developmental period. The study's unique sample of active offenders allows for a more nuanced understanding of how experiences with the justice system shape legal socialization.

II. LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

The concept of legal socialization is intertwined with broader notions of fairness, justice, punishment, and criminal responsibility. This study draws upon Tyler's theory of compliance and legitimacy, which highlights the importance of perceived procedural justice in shaping individuals' attitudes towards legal authorities.

Two key dimensions of legal socialization are examined: institutional legitimacy and legal cynicism. Legitimacy refers to the sense of obligation to defer to the decisions and rules of legal institutions and actors. It reflects a willingness to prioritize the authority's judgment over personal interests and moral values. Legal cynicism, on the other hand, reflects general views about the legitimacy of social norms and the law, reflecting a sense that these rules are not binding in everyday life.

A. THE LONGITUDINAL PATTERNING OF LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

This study posits that both legitimacy and legal cynicism are likely to vary over time within individuals, especially during adolescence. This assertion is supported by prior research on deterrence and compliance. The deterrence literature suggests that perceptions of sanction threats, a key factor in compliance, are not stable over time. Similarly, the compliance literature, while largely cross-sectional, demonstrates the importance of legal socialization in shaping adult behavior, highlighting the need for longitudinal studies to understand the developmental processes involved.

B. PRIOR RESEARCH ON LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

Existing research, while not longitudinal, offers valuable insights into the relationship between legal socialization and law-abiding behavior. Studies have examined neighborhood-level factors influencing legal cynicism, the role of procedural justice in shaping compliance, and the association between both legitimacy and legal cynicism with self-reported delinquency. However, these studies are limited by their cross-sectional design and focus on general population samples, hindering a comprehensive understanding of legal socialization as a developmental process.

III. CURRENT STUDY

This study analyzes data from a longitudinal study of serious adolescent offenders, examining the developmental trajectories of legal cynicism and legitimacy over an eighteen-month period. The use of trajectory-based models allows for a detailed examination of within-person variability in these attitudes over time.

A. DATA

The study utilizes data from the Pathways to Desistance study, a large-scale longitudinal study of adjudicated adolescents in Philadelphia and Phoenix. The sample consists of 1355 individuals, with data collected at four time points over eighteen months.

1. Variables of Interest

The study examines two key variables: legal cynicism and legitimacy. Legal cynicism is measured using a five-item scale assessing respondents' agreement with statements suggesting that laws are meant to be broken, that there are no right or wrong ways to make money, and that individuals should live without thinking about the future. Legitimacy is measured using an eleven-item scale assessing respondents' respect for and support of the police and courts, as well as their perceptions of the fairness of the legal system.

2. Methods

Trajectory-based models, a group-based approach, are employed to analyze the within-person variability in legal cynicism and legitimacy across time. These models identify subgroups of individuals with distinct developmental trajectories.

B. RESULTS

1. Legal Cynicism Trajectories:

The best-fitting model for legal cynicism identified four distinct trajectory groups. Two groups showed stable levels of cynicism over time, while a third group exhibited a slight increase in cynicism. The fourth group, comprising a small percentage of the sample, showed a more pronounced increase in cynicism over time. The analysis revealed high mean assignment probabilities, indicating that the majority of individuals could be classified into a particular trajectory group with high confidence.

2. What Factors Relate to Legal Cynicism?

The four trajectory groups differed significantly in terms of several baseline characteristics. Individuals in the higher cynicism groups had a higher number of prior arrests, were more likely to be Hispanic, and had lower procedural justice perceptions, particularly regarding court procedures.

3. Legitimacy Trajectories:

A five-trajectory model was identified as the best fit for the legitimacy data. The model revealed two groups with stable trajectories, a third group showing a slight convergence in their perceptions over time, and a fourth group showing a slight divergence. The fifth group, comprising a small percentage of the sample, showed a significant increase in legitimacy over time. Similar to the legal cynicism analysis, the mean assignment probabilities for each group were high.

4. What Factors Relate to Legitimacy?

The five trajectory groups differed significantly in terms of age, site, prior arrests, race, lock-up status, and procedural justice perceptions. Older individuals, African Americans, and individuals who were locked up tended to have lower legitimacy perceptions. Individuals in Phoenix had higher legitimacy perceptions than those in Philadelphia. Procedural justice perceptions regarding both police and court procedures were significantly associated with higher legitimacy perceptions.

5. Concordance Between Legal Cynicism and Legitimacy Trajectories

The analysis revealed a strong negative association between legal cynicism and legitimacy. Individuals with the lowest legal cynicism scores tended to have the highest legitimacy scores, while those with the highest cynicism scores had the lowest legitimacy scores.

6. Alternative Stratified Trajectory Specifications:

Supplemental analyses examining stratified trajectory models based on prior arrests, lock-up status, and age further confirmed the stability of legal cynicism and legitimacy over time, although different baseline levels were observed for the different subgroups.

C. DISCUSSION

This study provides the first longitudinal investigation of legal cynicism and legitimacy among serious juvenile offenders. The key findings indicate that these attitudes are highly stable over time, although there are significant differences in mean levels across individuals.

Factors such as prior arrests, race, procedural justice perceptions, and site location were found to influence the overall level of legal cynicism and legitimacy.

The study's findings highlight the importance of early experiences and socialization processes in shaping attitudes towards the law. The stability of these attitudes suggests that interventions aimed at promoting positive legal socialization should focus on addressing these early influences and promoting positive experiences with the legal system.

The study's limitations include the use of a trajectory methodology, which relies on the assumption of a discrete distribution of unobserved heterogeneity. Further, the eighteen-month time frame of the study may not be sufficient to fully capture changes in attitudes. Future research should explore these limitations and investigate the influence of pre- and post-adolescent characteristics on legal socialization trajectories.

Despite these limitations, this study provides valuable insights into the development of legal cynicism and legitimacy among serious juvenile offenders, highlighting the enduring nature of these attitudes and the critical role of early experiences in shaping them.

Summary

Legal socialization is the process of developing beliefs and attitudes about laws, courts, and legal authorities. It is important because adults' beliefs about the law's legitimacy affect how they behave. This study analyzes data from serious adolescent offenders to examine the development of two legal socialization dimensions: legal cynicism and legitimacy.

I. Introduction

Most research about legal socialization focuses on adults' views, but little is known about how it happens in adolescence. This is important because this is the time when individuals are developing an adult understanding of society and its institutions.

This study examines the development of legal cynicism and legitimacy in adolescent offenders because this group is more likely to have contact with the legal system and therefore may have different socialization patterns.

II. LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

The study's understanding of legal socialization is rooted in larger societal ideas about fairness, justice, punishment, and criminal responsibility. This study focuses on the two dimensions of legal socialization most relevant to adolescent offenders: institutional legitimacy and legal cynicism.

  • Institutional legitimacy: People feel obligated to follow the rules and decisions of legal institutions and actors.

  • Legal cynicism: People believe that laws are not binding and feel that breaking laws is justified.

These two dimensions are important to consider because they are related to self-definition during adolescence and the development of antisocial behavior.

A. THE LONGITUDINAL PATTERNING OF LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

It is reasonable to expect these dimensions to change over time, especially during childhood and adolescence. Research in deterrence and compliance supports this assumption.

  • Deterrence: Studies show that individuals' perception of the likelihood of being caught for committing an offense (sanction threat perceptions) varies over time.

  • Compliance: Studies show that adults who feel more obligated to obey the law are more likely to comply with the law and cooperate with legal authorities. However, most of this research is cross-sectional, comparing individuals of different ages, rather than following individuals over time.

B. PRIOR RESEARCH ON LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

Existing research, though not longitudinal, has highlighted important aspects of the relationship between legal socialization and law-abiding behavior.

  • Neighborhood-level factors: Studies have shown that the structural characteristics of neighborhoods (like concentrated disadvantage) can affect legal cynicism and other attitudes, especially among racial minorities.

  • Experiences with legal authorities: Studies have shown that recent personal experiences with the police and courts, and views of the legitimacy of the police and courts, affect compliance with the law. Procedural justice, or the perception of fairness in legal processes, influences perceptions of legitimacy.

  • Delinquency: Studies have shown that both legitimacy and legal cynicism predict self-reported delinquency, and that perceptions of fairness and respect from legal actors are related to these dimensions of legal socialization.

Despite the importance of this research, it is limited by being largely cross-sectional and often using general population samples, which may not reflect the experiences of individuals who are more frequently involved with the legal system.

III. CURRENT STUDY

This study examines the development of legal cynicism and legitimacy among serious adolescent offenders. The study uses data from four waves of interviews over eighteen months to identify and examine variation in the developmental trajectories of these two dimensions of legal socialization.

A. DATA

The data come from the Pathways to Desistance study, which follows adjudicated adolescents between the ages of fourteen and eighteen. The sample includes adolescents from Philadelphia and Phoenix who were charged with serious crimes. Retention rates for the study were high, making it a valuable resource for examining legal socialization over time.

1. Variables of Interest

  • Legal Cynicism: This variable measures the extent to which respondents agree with statements that indicate a belief that laws are not binding and that breaking laws is justified.

  • Legitimacy: This variable measures the extent to which respondents agree with statements that indicate respect for and obligation to legal authorities and institutions.

2. Methods

This study employs trajectory-based models to examine within-person variability across time in legal cynicism and legitimacy. These models identify groups of individuals who share similar developmental trajectories.

B. RESULTS

  1. Legal Cynicism Trajectories: The study identified four groups of adolescents with distinct legal cynicism trajectories:

    • Group 1: Stable and low legal cynicism.

    • Group 2: Stable and moderate legal cynicism.

    • Group 3: Stable and high legal cynicism.

    • Group 4: Increasing legal cynicism over time.

  2. Factors Related to Legal Cynicism: Several factors were found to be associated with the different legal cynicism trajectories, including:

    • Site: Philadelphia participants were more likely to be in the high cynicism group than Phoenix participants.

    • Prior Arrests: Participants with more prior arrests were more likely to be in the high cynicism groups.

    • Race: Hispanic juveniles were more likely to report higher legal cynicism than white juveniles.

    • Gender: Females were less likely to report high cynicism scores than males.

    • Procedural Justice: Participants with higher perceptions of procedural justice in court settings were less likely to report high levels of legal cynicism.

  3. Legitimacy Trajectories: The study identified five groups of adolescents with distinct legitimacy trajectories:

    • Group 1: Stable and low legitimacy.

    • Group 2: Stable and moderate legitimacy.

    • Group 3: Stable and high legitimacy.

    • Group 4: Decreasing legitimacy over time.

    • Group 5: Increasing legitimacy over time.

  4. Factors Related to Legitimacy: Several factors were found to be associated with the different legitimacy trajectories, including:

    • Age: Older individuals were less likely to perceive the law as legitimate.

    • Site: Phoenix juveniles were more likely to have higher perceptions of legitimacy than Philadelphia juveniles.

    • Race: African-American juveniles were more likely to report lower legitimacy perceptions than white juveniles.

    • Lock-up Status: Juveniles who were locked-up were more likely to have lower perceptions of legitimacy.

    • Gender: Females were more likely to report higher legitimacy perceptions than males.

    • Procedural Justice: Participants with higher perceptions of procedural justice in both police and court settings were more likely to report higher levels of legitimacy.

  5. Concordance Between Legal Cynicism and Legitimacy Trajectories: As expected, individuals with lower levels of legal cynicism were more likely to perceive the law as legitimate.

C. DISCUSSION

The study's findings show that legal cynicism and legitimacy are highly stable during adolescence, even among serious offenders. While there is little change in these dimensions over time, there are differences in mean levels of legal cynicism and legitimacy that are associated with factors such as:

  • Prior arrests: Higher cynicism.

  • Race: Hispanic youth report higher cynicism than white youth.

  • Age: Older individuals report lower legitimacy than younger individuals.

  • Race: African-American youth report lower legitimacy than white youth.

  • Lock-up status: Youth who have been locked-up report lower legitimacy.

These findings suggest that individuals' beliefs about the law are shaped by various factors and may be relatively stable even after multiple contacts with the legal system.

This study has important implications for understanding how legal socialization develops during adolescence, and how it is related to criminal behavior.

Summary

This study looks at how teenagers' feelings about the law change over time. It focuses on young people who have been in trouble with the law. The researchers found that, in general, teens' feelings about the law were pretty steady even over a long period. They also found that factors like having been in trouble with the law before and being from a certain race can affect how someone feels about the law.

I. Introduction

People learn about the law and how it works from their experiences. These experiences can be personal, like getting in trouble with the police, or from things they see or hear about. Most of what we know about how people learn about the law comes from studying adults. We know that adults' feelings about the law are important because they can affect how people behave.

It's important to study how teenagers learn about the law because they are starting to become adults and are experiencing more of the world. Teenagers are starting to understand the rules and how they are enforced.

II. LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

There are two important ways that teens learn about the law.

  • Legitimacy: Legitimacy is how much someone believes in the law and thinks it's fair. When someone feels a strong sense of legitimacy, they are more likely to follow the law.

  • Cynicism: Cynicism is when someone doesn't believe in the law or thinks it's unfair. Someone who is cynical is more likely to break the law.

A. THE LONGITUDINAL PATTERNING OF LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

These feelings about the law can change over time, especially during childhood and adolescence. For example, some people might become more cynical over time, while others might become more trusting of the law. This is important to understand because it can affect how people behave.

B. PRIOR RESEARCH ON LEGAL SOCIALIZATION

There are several studies that show how people's feelings about the law are connected to how they behave. For example, people who believe the law is fair are more likely to follow the law. Other studies show that people who have had bad experiences with the law are more likely to be cynical.

III. CURRENT STUDY

This study looks at a group of teenagers who have been in trouble with the law. They were interviewed over a period of 18 months. The researchers wanted to see if their feelings about the law changed over time.

A. DATA

The study included over 1,300 teenagers in two cities. The teens were all involved in the juvenile justice system. The researchers interviewed the teenagers four times over the course of the study.

1. Variables of Interest

  • Legal Cynicism: The researchers measured how cynical the teenagers were about the law. They asked questions like "Laws are meant to be broken" and "There are no right or wrong ways to make money."

  • Legitimacy: The researchers measured how much the teenagers believed in the law. They asked questions like "I have a great deal of respect for the police" and "The courts generally guarantee everyone a fair hearing."

2. Methods

The researchers used a special kind of math to track how teens' feelings about the law changed over time. They found that the teenagers could be divided into groups based on how their feelings about the law changed.

B. RESULTS

  1. Legal Cynicism Trajectories: The researchers found that most teenagers' cynicism about the law stayed about the same over time. There were a few groups that showed more change.

  2. What Factors Relate to Legal Cynicism? The researchers found that several factors were connected to how cynical teenagers were, including:

    • Their race

    • Whether they had been in trouble with the law before

    • Their experiences with the police and courts

  3. Legitimacy Trajectories: The researchers found that most teenagers' beliefs in the law stayed about the same over time. There were a few groups that showed more change.

  4. What Factors Relate to Legitimacy? The researchers found that several factors were connected to how much teenagers believed in the law, including:

    • Their age

    • Their race

    • Whether they had been in trouble with the law before

    • Their experiences with the police and courts

  5. Concordance Between Legal Cynicism and Legitimacy Trajectories The researchers found that teens who were more cynical about the law were less likely to believe in the law.

  6. Alternative Stratified Trajectory Specifications The researchers also looked at how these feelings about the law changed in different groups of teens, such as those who had been in trouble with the law before or those who were locked up. They found that teens in all these groups tended to have stable feelings about the law over time.

C. DISCUSSION

This study found that teenagers' feelings about the law are pretty stable over time. The researchers also found that several factors can influence how teens feel about the law. They concluded that it is important to study how teens learn about the law so that we can understand why they might break the law.

This study is important because it helps us understand why teens might break the law. It also shows that teens' feelings about the law are often shaped by their experiences with the police and courts.

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Piquero, A. R., Fagan, J., Mulvey, E. P., Steinberg, L., & Odgers, C. (2005). Developmental trajectories of legal socialization among serious adolescent offenders. The Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 96(1), 267.

    Highlights