Current Understanding of Developmental Changes in Adolescent Perspective Taking
Nikki C. Lee
Miriam Hollarek
SummaryOriginal

Summary

This study on adolescent perspective-taking shows that gender, family, and school influence development. More research is needed for deeper understanding and better measurement methods.

2022

Current Understanding of Developmental Changes in Adolescent Perspective Taking

Keywords perspective taking; adolescence; development; mentalizing; cognitive empathy

Abstract

Perspective taking is an essential skill for social development during adolescence and understanding its development could provide important insights into adolescent well-being. We review recent longitudinal and cross-sectional work that demonstrates continued refinement of perspective-taking abilities during adolescence, based on both self-report measures and (advanced) perspective-taking tasks. We discuss factors that contribute to individual differences in perspective-taking abilities, such as gender, parent and peer interactions, and the school environment. The reviewed studies highlight the need for further longitudinal studies, which identify factors that promote or hinder the development of perspective taking during the adolescent period and use developmentally appropriate measures, which can assess advanced perspective-taking abilities.

Introduction

A critical skill for successful social interactions is the ability to take someone else's perspective [1]. This is especially important during adolescence, when a key developmental task is the formation of lasting positive relationships outside the primary family. In order to navigate their increasingly complex social environments, adolescents need to develop advanced social cognitive competencies [2]. Well-developed perspective-taking abilities have been found to improve social relationships [3,4], and are related to higher friendship quality and lower levels of loneliness [5]. Impaired perspective-taking abilities have been associated with various types of psychopathology such as internalising and externalising problems [6], adolescent-onset schizophrenia [7], as well as a wide range of mental disorders in adults [8]. A thorough understanding of the development of perspective taking may therefore elucidate ways to improve adolescents' social connectedness, which is vital to their mental well-being [9].

Perspective taking has been defined as the ability to understand and reason about or predict other people's thoughts, beliefs, mental states, or emotions, and is also referred to as cognitive empathy, cognitive theory of mind, or mentalising [10, 11, 12]. Perspective taking is often measured together with an affective component (known as affective empathy or affective theory of mind), which refers to one's own emotional response to other's feelings. These two components are separate processes with their own developmental trajectories [13], and the current review focuses solely on the development of perspective taking, that is, the cognitive process of understanding another's point of view. Most previous research on perspective taking has focused on childhood, despite important developmental changes during adolescence, which impact social interactions, such as hormonal changes during puberty and the resulting social re-orientation towards peers [4]. Ongoing development of perspective-taking abilities during adolescence is in line with neuroimaging studies suggesting that the brain areas involved in social cognitive processes, such as perspective taking, often referred to as the ‘social brain’, are undergoing substantial structural and functional change during this period [14]. For example, higher levels of perspective taking during adolescence are associated with relatively more mature cortical structures in these brain regions, compared to development in those with lower levels of perspective taking [15].

This review highlights recent advances in the study of the development of perspective taking during adolescence. We first review research describing the overall developmental trajectory of perspective-taking abilities, including gender differences. Next, we discuss work aiming to elucidate individual differences in the development of perspective taking, specifically the influence of parent-child and peer interactions, as well as socio-environmental factors. We conclude by providing recommendations for future research.

Developmental trajectory

The majority of research on the development of perspective-taking abilities has focused on the initial acquisition of this ability during childhood. Young children develop basic aspects of perspective taking, such as an awareness of mental states in themselves and others, followed by an understanding of other people's beliefs [16]. Early development of perspective taking is predicted by functions facilitating shared experiences, such as attention, inhibitory control, and language abilities (for a review on precursors of perspective taking in childhood see the study by Derksen et al. [4]). It has been proposed that differences in language abilities in particular may contribute to early individual differences in perspective-taking abilities [17,18]. This in line with twin studies showing that the same genetic factors that influence individual differences in perspective taking were also related to language abilities [19,20]. The modest heritability of perspective-taking abilities is presumed to further decrease as the social and environmental factors become more influential in later developmental stages [17].

In contrast to childhood, adolescence appears to be a period of refinement of perspective taking, where more advanced socio-cognitive skills are required to meet the demands of the increasingly complex social interactions that adolescents encounter [21]. Recent longitudinal studies provide strong evidence for continued advancement of perspective taking during adolescence, reporting an overall increase from age 13–18 [1]. One study from a non-western background provides initial evidence that this increase in perspective taking during adolescence can be observed across cultures [22]. While language abilities seem to become less important, executive functions, such as inhibitory control and set shifting, continue to be related to perspective-taking abilities during adolescence and adulthood [21,23, 24, 25].

The vast majority of studies in children have used false belief tasks to measure perspective taking [16], yet performance on such lower level perspective taking tasks show ceiling effects in late childhood [26,27]. Most studies investigating perspective-taking abilities in adolescents used self-report measures [28, 29, 30, 31], specifically the perspective taking subscale of the interpersonal reactivity index [10]. Some work has been done using tasks designed to measure advanced perspective taking, which tap into more complex mental state attribution using stories or films, or ask participants to perform a task while taking someone else's visual perspective into account. These tasks enable investigation of the nuances of more complex perspective taking, and as with self-report measures, generally show protracted development from early to late adolescence [27,32, ∗33, 34]. However, the psychometric properties of tasks trying to capture more advanced perspective taking skills have not yet been fully examined [35], and commonly used measures in children have shown poor coherence with measures used in adults [36]. More research is needed to clarify which aspects of perspective taking each task measures, and how these compare across tasks and questionnaires. An overview of the most commonly used measures (including a short description) is given in Table 1.

Reading the mind in the eyes task (RME)

Participants need to recognise complex emotional and cognitive states just by looking at pictures of eyes.

Białecka-Pikul et al., 2020

Movie for the assessment of social cognition (MASC)

Participants watch short films and answer questions regarding the actors' mental states.

Boada et al., 2020

Edinburgh social cognition test (ESCoT)

Participants watch cartoon-style social interactions and answer questions about the cognitive and affective states of the characters, asl well as and inter- and intrapersonal understanding of social norms.

Baksh et al., 2018

Movie for assessment of empathy and ToM (EmpaToM-Y)

Participants watch short films and answer questions entailing mental perspective taking and give affect ratings of the depicted scenes.

Breil et al., 2021

Director task

Participants need to take another person's perspective to perform an action.

Humphrey and Dumontheil, 2016

Cartoon vignettes

Participants need to choose between endings to cartoon vignettes.

Gao et al., 2019

ToM stories for children and adolescents

Participants read stories and answer questions about basic and higher order false belief reasoning.

Gabriel et al., 2021

Self-report questionnaires

Interpersonal reactivity index

Self-report measure assessing four subscales of empathy: Perspective taking, empathic concern, personal distress, and fantasy.

Farrell and Vaillancourt, 2021

Adolescent measure of empathy and sympathy (AMES)

Self-report measure designed for adolescents assessing the subscales cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and sympathy.

Li et al., 2019

Basic empathy scale

Self-report measure assessing cognitive and affective empathy.

Zych et al., 2020

Table 1. Overview of commonly used measures to assess advanced perspective taking in adolescents

Gender differences1

Gender differences in perspective taking have been found using a multitude of measures [5,42], although most consistently reported in studies using self-reports [28,31,45,46]. These studies show that girls have higher levels of perspective-taking abilities than boys already at the age of 13, and this difference continues to grow until age 18 due to a steeper increase in this ability among girls. Boys on the other hand show a decrease in perspective taking from early to middle adolescence followed by an increase [31].

Most explanations of gender differences in perspective taking have focused on socialisation effects. While theoretical accounts also propose that puberty influences socio-cognitive abilities, to date there is still limited empirical evidence for pubertal effects on perspective taking [47]. Socialisation theories propose that due to gender specific societal demands, girls are often socialised to be more nurturing and expressive [31], which may both encourage the use of perspective taking abilities, as well as foster their development. In contrast, boys are often expected to behave in a more assertive and competitive manner [48]. Adherence to gender roles is particularly strong during mid adolescence [31], suggesting that the decrease in perspective taking in mid-adolescent boys may be a consequence of socially desirable behaviour. In line with this hypothesis, studies found that the decrease observed in boys in mid-adolescence was particularly prevalent among those with more stereotypical ideas about gender roles [49]. Moreover, observed gender differences in the cognitive process of perspective taking are often smaller than those reported for the more affective components, such as affective empathy [22,31,50], and some studies have only found significant gender differences in affective empathy, but not in perspective taking [41,51]. The finding of larger gender differences on self-reported affective empathy than cognitive components of perspective taking provide further support for socialisation theories, as it may be less socially desirable for boys to report high affect [49]. It may be that the ability to take another person's perspective is actually similar for boys and girls, but the propensity to apply this ability may decrease in boys, particularly during adolescence when social status and societal expectations are an important driver of behaviour [5].

Individual differences

As perspective taking is a vital skill for successful social relationships, numerous lines of work have focused on elucidating the factors (in addition to gender) which influence its development. This is of particular relevance during adolescence, when the ability to understand and sensitively respond to the needs and desires of others is a prerequisite for adolescents to form the friendships and romantic attachments, which are essential to their social development [52]. Recent work investigating individual differences in perspective taking has demonstrated heterogeneity in developmental trajectories [30], emphasising the need for research into the factors that may support or constrain improvements in these abilities. Among the most frequently investigated factors are parent and peer interactions, which we discuss below, as well as the contributions of the broader social environment.

Parents

One line of work has focused on how the dynamics of parent–child interactions influence the development of perspective taking. A recent meta-analysis found that adolescents with higher quality relationships with their parents showed higher levels of perspective taking [53]. This is in line with socialisation theories, which suggest that within supportive relationships parents’ model caring behaviours such as considering the thoughts and intentions of others, thereby fostering these abilities in their children [48]. Parents may also be able to actively encourage perspective taking among their children, for example by rewarding considerate behaviour, with recent work showing that social rewards, such as praise are more effective in achieving this than material rewards [54]. Others have found increases in perspective taking as a result of parents directly encouraging social responsibility [55].

Interactions with parents also provide a safe space to learn conflict resolution skills, as relationships with parents can be used as a model for later relationships with peers [56]. Adolescents who are unable to have these learning experiences, for example, due to parental maltreatment, show slower development of perspective-taking abilities [57]. Multiple studies reveal the importance of perspective taking in successfully navigating parent–child conflict, as adolescents with higher levels of perspective taking show more interest in their mother's thoughts during a conflict discussion [58] as well increased sensitivity in adapting their behaviour to their mother's responses [59]. Since many past studies have only included mothers, further research is needed to discern the role of fathers and other primary caregivers on adolescents' perspective taking. In a promising finding for future interventions, a manipulation that primed perspective taking led to less escalation during conflicts between adolescents and their mothers and increased their attention to their mother's point of view [60].

Peers

Others have focused on the influence of friends and peers. As they develop into independent adults, adolescents spend increasing amounts of time with their friends and less with their parents, and the focus of their emotional support shifts from parents to peers [52]. As with parents, perspective taking is positively associated with the quality of adolescent peer relationships, and the association with peer relationships is in fact stronger than that with parents [48]. Similar effects were found for related constructs, including peer attachment and closeness, supporting the proposition that higher quality peer relationships are characterised by higher levels of perspective taking [61]. Peer relationships may not only provide socialisation of perspective taking though modelling of behaviours, but increased perspective taking is also thought to facilitate the development of these relationships [48]. While relationships with parents are relatively stable and involuntary, those with peers develop voluntarily and consequently an adolescent's ability to understand and consider the thoughts of others may determine how well they are able to form friendships. Interestingly, these effects may be strongest at the level of individual relationships, as a meta-analysis was unable to demonstrate an effect of perspective taking on social status variables such as popularity or likeability [61].

Socio-environmental factors

Social influences have also been observed outside parent and peer relationships. These lines of work show a bidirectional relationship between the social environment and perspective taking. While social factors (such as the classroom environment or norms within a friendship group) can shape the development of perspective taking abilities, an individual adolescent's attained level of perspective taking can also impact their behaviour during interactions with others. Studies focussing on the former have shown that pupils who are encouraged by teachers to express their opinions show improved levels of perspective taking [55] and that perspective taking is positively associated with social competence in the classroom [62]. Recent work during the COVID-19 pandemic found that perspective taking increased during the first weeks of lockdown, with the authors suggesting that this extreme situation encouraged adolescents to use their perspective-taking abilities to reflect on the circumstances others found themselves in the study by Portt et al [63]. Other work has focused on how perspective-taking skills impact adolescents' social behaviour, with multiple lines of research suggesting that perspective taking affects how adolescents relate to and behave towards others. Recent work has shown that higher levels of perspective taking predict lower levels of prejudice [64], more restorative opinions about punishment [65] and led adolescents to make larger donations when sharing with others, particularly strangers [63]. These findings align with the extensive evidence for perspective taking as an important determinant of prosocial behaviour [53]. An increased awareness of the other's feelings and intentions is thought to promote prosocial actions, such as finding ways to help others.

Future directions and conclusions

In summary, recent findings have shown that perspective-taking abilities continue to be refined during the adolescent period, and individual differences in these trajectories are shaped by a number of factors including gender and social interactions with parents, peers, and the broader social environment. However, there are a number of challenges that future research should address. Firstly, it is pivotal to find and validate suitable measures of advanced perspective-taking abilities. Ideally such measures should be able to capture subtle changes in perspective taking during the adolescent period and should be suitable for repeated administration and neuroimaging studies. Moreover, to decrease susceptibility to social desirability and to increase ecological validity, longitudinal research needs to expand from using (sub-scales of) self-report measures towards assessing realistic social scenarios or real life interactions. Secondly, while a number of longitudinal studies have adequately addressed changes in perspective taking during adolescence, not enough is currently known about longitudinal effects of precursors and outcomes of perspective-taking abilities, as well as their potential interplay over time [4]. Studies employing appropriate measures longitudinally over the period of adolescence will help to elucidate the factors which affect individual differences in the developmental trajectory of perspective-taking abilities. By better identifying these factors that promote or hinder the development of perspective taking, we may be able to prevent adverse developmental outcomes and foster adolescents’ social functioning and mental well-being.

Link to Article

Abstract

Perspective taking is an essential skill for social development during adolescence and understanding its development could provide important insights into adolescent well-being. We review recent longitudinal and cross-sectional work that demonstrates continued refinement of perspective-taking abilities during adolescence, based on both self-report measures and (advanced) perspective-taking tasks. We discuss factors that contribute to individual differences in perspective-taking abilities, such as gender, parent and peer interactions, and the school environment. The reviewed studies highlight the need for further longitudinal studies, which identify factors that promote or hinder the development of perspective taking during the adolescent period and use developmentally appropriate measures, which can assess advanced perspective-taking abilities.

The Development of Perspective Taking in Adolescence

Developmental Trajectory

Perspective taking, the cognitive ability to understand others' mental states, continues to develop during adolescence. Longitudinal studies indicate an overall increase in perspective-taking abilities from ages 13 to 18. This refinement is likely due to the increasing complexity of social interactions encountered during this period.

Gender Differences

Studies consistently report higher perspective-taking abilities in girls compared to boys, with the difference widening during adolescence. Socialization theories suggest that gender-specific societal expectations may contribute to this disparity.

Individual Differences

Parent-Child Interactions

Higher-quality parent-child relationships are associated with higher levels of perspective taking in adolescents. Parents may foster these abilities through modeling considerate behavior and encouraging social responsibility.

Peer Interactions

Perspective taking is also positively associated with the quality of peer relationships. Adolescents with higher perspective-taking abilities may be better equipped to form and maintain friendships.

Socio-Environmental Factors

The social environment can influence perspective-taking development. Classroom environments that encourage expression of opinions and social competence have been linked to improved perspective taking. Additionally, perspective-taking abilities can impact adolescents' social behavior, leading to lower prejudice, more restorative views on punishment, and increased prosocial actions.

Future Directions and Conclusions

Future research should focus on developing validated measures of advanced perspective-taking abilities and conducting longitudinal studies to investigate the interplay of factors influencing its developmental trajectory. By identifying factors that promote or hinder perspective-taking development, interventions can be designed to enhance adolescents' social functioning and well-being.

Link to Article

Abstract

Perspective taking is an essential skill for social development during adolescence and understanding its development could provide important insights into adolescent well-being. We review recent longitudinal and cross-sectional work that demonstrates continued refinement of perspective-taking abilities during adolescence, based on both self-report measures and (advanced) perspective-taking tasks. We discuss factors that contribute to individual differences in perspective-taking abilities, such as gender, parent and peer interactions, and the school environment. The reviewed studies highlight the need for further longitudinal studies, which identify factors that promote or hinder the development of perspective taking during the adolescent period and use developmentally appropriate measures, which can assess advanced perspective-taking abilities.

The Development of Perspective Taking in Adolescence

Introduction

Perspective taking, or the ability to understand others' thoughts and feelings, is crucial for successful social interactions. This ability continues to develop during adolescence, a time of significant social changes. Understanding the factors that influence this development can help improve adolescents' social connections and well-being.

Developmental Trajectory

While basic perspective-taking skills emerge in childhood, adolescence is a period of refinement. Studies have shown an overall increase in perspective-taking abilities from ages 13 to 18. This development is influenced by ongoing brain maturation and the increasing complexity of social interactions.

Gender Differences

Girls tend to exhibit higher levels of perspective taking than boys, with the difference widening during adolescence. This may be due to societal expectations that encourage girls to be more nurturing and expressive, fostering the use and development of perspective-taking skills.

Individual Differences

Parent-Child Interactions:

  • High-quality parent-child relationships are associated with higher levels of perspective taking.

  • Parents model considerate behavior and encourage perspective taking through rewards and discussions.

  • Conflict resolution skills learned in parent-child interactions can transfer to peer relationships.

Peer Interactions:

  • Perspective taking is also positively associated with the quality of peer relationships.

  • Friendships provide opportunities to practice and develop perspective-taking skills.

  • Higher perspective taking may facilitate the formation of close friendships.

Socio-Environmental Factors:

  • Social environments, such as classrooms and friendship groups, can influence perspective taking.

  • Encouragement from teachers and positive social norms promote its development.

  • Perspective taking can also impact social behavior, leading to lower prejudice, more restorative attitudes, and increased prosocial actions.

Future Directions and Conclusions

Future research should focus on:

  • Developing reliable measures of advanced perspective-taking abilities.

  • Using longitudinal studies to investigate the long-term effects of factors influencing perspective taking.

  • Identifying interventions that can promote the development of perspective taking.

By understanding the factors that shape perspective-taking abilities, we can support adolescents' social development and enhance their overall well-being.

Link to Article

Abstract

Perspective taking is an essential skill for social development during adolescence and understanding its development could provide important insights into adolescent well-being. We review recent longitudinal and cross-sectional work that demonstrates continued refinement of perspective-taking abilities during adolescence, based on both self-report measures and (advanced) perspective-taking tasks. We discuss factors that contribute to individual differences in perspective-taking abilities, such as gender, parent and peer interactions, and the school environment. The reviewed studies highlight the need for further longitudinal studies, which identify factors that promote or hinder the development of perspective taking during the adolescent period and use developmentally appropriate measures, which can assess advanced perspective-taking abilities.

Understanding Others' Perspectives: A Key Skill for Teenagers

As you navigate the social world as a teenager, it's crucial to be able to understand other people's thoughts and feelings. This skill, called perspective taking, is essential for building strong relationships and getting along with others.

How Perspective Taking Develops

You've probably already developed some basic perspective-taking skills as a child. But during adolescence, these skills continue to grow and become more advanced. Studies have shown that teenagers' ability to understand others' perspectives improves from ages 13 to 18.

Why Gender Matters

Girls tend to have higher levels of perspective taking than boys, and this difference becomes more noticeable as they get older. This might be because of how society expects girls and boys to behave. Girls are often encouraged to be more caring and understanding, while boys are expected to be more competitive and assertive.

The Role of Parents and Peers

Your relationships with your parents and friends can also influence your perspective-taking skills. Teens who have close and supportive relationships with their parents tend to be better at understanding others' perspectives. Similarly, having high-quality friendships is linked to higher levels of perspective taking.

The Impact of Your Environment

The social environment you're in can also shape your perspective-taking abilities. For example, if your teachers encourage you to share your opinions, you're more likely to develop these skills. Additionally, being in situations that make you think about others' circumstances, like the COVID-19 pandemic, can also improve your perspective taking.

How Perspective Taking Affects Your Behavior

Being able to understand others' perspectives can have a positive impact on your behavior. It can help you be less prejudiced, more understanding of others' punishments, and more likely to help those in need.

Conclusion

Perspective taking is an important skill that continues to develop during adolescence. Your gender, relationships, and social environment can all influence your ability to understand others' perspectives. By improving your perspective-taking skills, you can build stronger relationships, get along better with others, and make a positive impact on the world around you.

Link to Article

Abstract

Perspective taking is an essential skill for social development during adolescence and understanding its development could provide important insights into adolescent well-being. We review recent longitudinal and cross-sectional work that demonstrates continued refinement of perspective-taking abilities during adolescence, based on both self-report measures and (advanced) perspective-taking tasks. We discuss factors that contribute to individual differences in perspective-taking abilities, such as gender, parent and peer interactions, and the school environment. The reviewed studies highlight the need for further longitudinal studies, which identify factors that promote or hinder the development of perspective taking during the adolescent period and use developmentally appropriate measures, which can assess advanced perspective-taking abilities.

Understanding Other People's Thoughts and Feelings

As you grow up, you get better at understanding other people's thoughts and feelings. This is called perspective taking. It's like being able to put yourself in someone else's shoes and see the world from their point of view.

How Perspective Taking Changes as You Get Older

When you're younger, you start to understand that other people have different thoughts and beliefs than you do. But as you get older, you get even better at understanding how others think and feel. This is especially true during your teenage years.

Why Perspective Taking is Important

Being able to understand other people's perspectives is really important for getting along with others. It helps you build strong friendships and relationships. When you can understand how someone else is feeling, you can be more kind and helpful to them.

What Helps You Get Better at Perspective Taking

There are a few things that can help you get better at perspective taking:

  • Your parents: When your parents talk to you about other people's feelings and thoughts, it helps you learn how to understand them.

  • Your friends: Spending time with friends and talking to them about different things helps you see the world from different perspectives.

  • Your environment: The things you see and experience in your daily life can also help you understand how others think and feel.

How Perspective Taking Helps You

When you're good at perspective taking, it can help you in many ways:

  • You can make better decisions because you can understand how your actions might affect others.

  • You can be more tolerant of people who are different from you.

  • You can be more helpful and supportive to others.

Link to Article

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Hollarek, M., & Lee, N. C. (2022). Current understanding of developmental changes in adolescent perspective taking. Current Opinion in Psychology, 45, 101308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101308

    Highlights