Behavioral and Neural Signatures of Working Memory in Childhood
Monica D. Rosenberg
Steven A. Martinez
Kristina M. Rapuano
May I. Conley
Alexandra O. Cohen
SummaryOriginal

Summary

Working memory (WM), which varies individually, is linked to short-term memory, language, and intelligence in children aged 9-10. Brain regions involved in WM are more active in children with stronger WM.

2020

Behavioral and Neural Signatures of Working Memory in Childhood

Keywords Development; Brain Scans; Childhood; Intelligence; Working memory; fMRI; frontoparietal; n-back

Abstract

Working memory function changes across development and varies across individuals. The patterns of behavior and brain function that track individual differences in working memory during human development, however, are not well understood. Here, we establish associations between working memory, other cognitive abilities, and functional MRI (fMRI) activation in data from over 11,500 9- to 10-year-old children (both sexes) enrolled in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, an ongoing longitudinal study in the United States. Behavioral analyses reveal robust relationships between working memory, short-term memory, language skills, and fluid intelligence. Analyses relating out-of-scanner working memory performance to memory-related fMRI activation in an emotional n-back task demonstrate that frontoparietal activity during a working memory challenge indexes working memory performance. This relationship is domain specific, such that fMRI activation related to emotion processing during the emotional n-back task, inhibitory control during a stop-signal task (SST), and reward processing during a monetary incentive delay (MID) task does not track memory abilities. Together, these results inform our understanding of individual differences in working memory in childhood and lay the groundwork for characterizing the ways in which they change across adolescence.

Introduction

Working memory, a collection of cognitive processes responsible for storing and manipulating information, is a foundational ability that varies widely across individuals. Individual differences in working memory, which appear to be stable over time (Alp, 1994; Ross et al., 2008; Johnson et al., 2013; Tulsky et al., 2014; Xu et al., 2018), have pronounced real-world significance. Although the direction of causality is unclear, measures of verbal and visuospatial working memory explain ;20–30% of the variance in fluid intelligence in children (Engel de Abreu et al., 2010), and visuospatial working memory performance explains .40% of this variance in adults (Fukuda et al., 2010). Furthermore, working memory function, which is related to executive and visuospatial attention (Kane and Engle, 2002; Huang et al., 2012), short-term memory (Alloway et al., 2006), and inhibitory control (Davidson et al., 2006), predicts consequential outcomes in development, including reading and math skills (Bayliss et al., 2003; De Smedt et al., 2009; Alloway and Alloway, 2010; Nouwens et al., 2017). Despite the theoretical and practical importance of characterizing associations between working memory and other mental processes, much remains to be learned about the nature of these relationships during development.

Working memory not only varies across individuals, but also changes across the lifespan. Working memory emerges in infancy and develops rapidly over the first year of life (Diamond and Goldman-Rakic, 1989; Ross-Sheehy et al., 2003; Reynolds and Romano, 2016; Buss et al., 2018). This ability continues to improve during childhood, plateaus in mid-to-late adolescence (Gathercole et al., 2004; Luciana et al., 2005; Conklin et al., 2007; Ullman et al., 2014; Isbell et al., 2015), and declines after age 40– 50, albeit less steeply than it changed during early development (Alloway and Alloway, 2013; Nyberg et al., 2014; Eriksson et al., 2015; Swanson, 2017). Developmental gains in working memory follow improvements in attention shifting, attentional maintenance, and distractor suppression (Reynolds and Romano, 2016), whereas changes during later childhood accompany increases in domain-general processing speed and memory capacity (Fry and Hale, 1996, 2000; Pailian et al., 2016) with developmental asymptotic performance by adolescence (Casey et al., 2000, 2005; Klingberg et al., 2002; Steinberg et al., 2009). Decrements in older adulthood relate to declines in processing speed, selective attention, and distractor suppression (Salthouse and Babcock, 1991; Gazzaley et al., 2005; McNab et al., 2015).

Converging neuroimaging evidence suggests that variation in frontoparietal brain systems, which are involved in processes including attention and cognitive control (Woolgar et al., 2011; Scolari et al., 2015; Assem et al., 2020), accounts for both developmental change in verbal and visuospatial working memory and individual differences in these processes in adulthood. Early work demonstrated that the same aspects of middle and inferior frontal cortex that support working memory performance in adults also support performance in children (Casey et al., 1995). This evidence led to theorizing that the protracted fine-tuning of prefrontal circuitry contributes to working memory improvements during childhood and adolescence (Casey et al., 2000, 2005). Longitudinal studies support this prediction, with evidence that maturation in prefrontal and parietal volume and structural connectivity accompany working memory development (Tamnes et al., 2013; Klingberg and Darki, 2015). Crosssectional work suggests that increases in frontoparietal activation during working memory tasks are associated with age-related improvements in performance (Klingberg et al., 2002; Kwon et al., 2002; Crone et al., 2006; Satterthwaite et al., 2013). In the developed brain, individual differences in frontoparietal areas’ microstructure, function, and structural and functional connectivity track individual differences in working memory (Osaka et al., 2003; Palva et al., 2010; Burzynska et al., 2011; Takeuchi et al., 2011; Ekman et al., 2016). A subset of developmental studies show similar associations between in-scanner working memory performance (a state-like measure of memory function) and frontoparietal activity during working memory tasks when controlling for age (Crone et al., 2006; Satterthwaite et al., 2013). One study comparing adolescents with higher and lower family incomes found that the higher-income group showed greater frontoparietal activity as a function of load in a working memory (n-back) functional MRI (fMRI) task and higher out-of-scanner working memory capacity (Finn et al., 2017a). However, it is not yet known whether frontoparietal network function during working memory challenges specifically, or during cognitive task challenges more generally, predicts individual differences in working memory during development.

Here, we examine behavioral and neural signatures of working memory in childhood. Using data from 11,537 9- to 10- year-olds participating in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentsM (ABCD) Study (Casey, 2018; Volkow et al., 2018), we first establish relationships between working memory and other cognitive and attentional abilities, including short-term memory, language and verbal skills, fluid intelligence, processing speed, attention, inhibitory control, and reward processing. Because the ABCD Study® will follow children longitudinally for 10 years, characterizing these associations in childhood not only informs the structure of cognition at a single time point but also facilitates understanding the ways in which this cognitive structure changes across adolescence. We next ask whether performance on an out-of-scanner working memory test is related to frontoparietal brain activity when measured (1) during a working memory challenge and (2) during task challenges unrelated to memory. Together, our results provide insight into individual differences in working memory in childhood, and underscore the importance of task fMRI as a “stress test” for cognition (Finn et al., 2017b) that can reveal task-specific and task-general neural signatures of a mental process or behavior.

Materials and Methods

The ABCD Study

Individual differences in working memory and other cognitive and attentional processes were assessed using data from 11,537 9- to 10-year-olds in the ABCD Study, an ongoing multisite longitudinal study of neurocognitive development (Luciana et al., 2018). Launched in September 2016, the ABCD Study aims to characterize cognitive and neural development with measures of neurocognition, physical and mental health, social and emotional function, and culture and environment. Exclusionary criteria include a diagnosis of schizophrenia, a moderate to severe autism spectrum disorder, an intellectual disability, or a substance use disorder at recruitment. Children with a persistent major neurologic disorder (e.g., cerebral palsy, a brain tumor, stroke, brain aneurysm, brain hemorrhage, subdural hematoma), multiple sclerosis, sickle cell disease, or certain seizure disorders (Lennox–Gastaut syndrome, Dravet syndrome, and Landau Kleffner syndrome) were also excluded.

ABCD Study data collection includes yearly behavioral assessments, interviews, questionnaires, and biosample collection as well as biennial MRI scans (Casey, 2018). Here, we analyze year-one (baseline) demographic and behavioral data collected across 22 sites when children were 9–10 years old and made available as part of curated data release 2.0.1 (DOI 10.15 154/1504041; n = 11,537; 48.4% female). Sample demographics including race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and symptoms of internalizing and externalizing disorders are available in Thompson et al. (2019).

Data from children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder or epilepsy were excluded from the current analysis because moderate to severe forms of autism spectrum disorder and other seizure disorders were exclusionary for the ABCD Study (n = 338 of 11,875). Data from children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, depression, bipolar disorder, anxiety, and phobias (n = 1,598 of 11,537) were not excluded, as these diagnoses were assessed with a single screening question and we aimed to characterize working memory in a heterogeneous developmental population.

Behavioral data

To characterize associations between working memory and other cognitive abilities, we analyzed performance data from all 15 available neurocognitive (Luciana et al., 2018) and neuroimaging (Casey, 2018) tasks, described in detail below (Table 1). Working memory ability outside of the scanner was operationalized as performance on the NIH Toolbox List Sorting Working Memory Task. Performance measures were selected based on previous work including reports of ABCD baseline data (Casey, 2018; Luciana et al., 2018).

NIH Toolbox cognition battery

The NIH Toolbox cognition battery includes seven tasks measuring multiple aspects of cognition (Gershon et al., 2013; Table 1, column 3). Performance is measured using uncorrected standard scores, as age-corrected scores are currently undergoing revision by the NIH Toolbox (Luciana et al., 2018).

The Toolbox List Sorting Working Memory Test measures working memory by asking children to recall stimuli in different orders (Tulsky et al., 2014). Children were first shown pictures of two animals and were asked to repeat them back in order from smallest to largest. They were shown longer lists (with up to seven animals) if they answered correctly. Next, children were shown pictures of both animals and foods and were asked to repeat the animals in order of size and then the foods in order of size. Interleaved lists increased in length from two to seven if children responded accurately, and performance scores reflect the number of accurate responses. Importantly, this list sorting task was developed to assess working memory function in general rather than verbal or visual working memory in particular (Tulsky et al., 2014). Visuospatial working memory was not measured in the ABCD baseline sample and reflects a cognitive ability closely related to but dissociable from verbal working memory (Alloway et al., 2006; Swanson, 2017) that is traditionally tested with tasks such as delayed match-to-sample.

The Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Test measures language and verbal abilities (Gershon et al., 2014). Children hear a series of words and are asked to choose which of four pictures most closely matches the meaning of the word.

The Toolbox Flanker Task, a flanker task (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974) used to measure cognitive control and attention, was adapted from the Attention Network Task (Fan et al., 2002; Zelazo et al., 2014). On each trial, children see a row of five arrows. The outer four arrows (distractors, or flankers) all point to the left or right of the screen. The middle arrow (the target) points in the same direction as the flankers on congruent trials, and the opposite direction of the flankers on incongruent trials. Children are asked to indicate whether the center arrow points to the left or to the right. Performance scores are based on speed and accuracy.

The Toolbox Dimensional Change Card Sort Task measures cognitive flexibility (Zelazo, 2006). On each trial, children see two objects on a screen. They are asked to match a third item with one of the initial two based on either color or shape. Children first match items based on one dimension (e.g., color), then match items based on the other dimension (e.g., shape), and finally match based on both shape and color in pseudorandom order. Performance scores are based on speed and accuracy.

The Toolbox Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test measures visual processing speed (Salthouse et al., 1991; Carlozzi et al., 2015). Children are shown two pictures and are asked to indicate whether they are the same or different. Scores are based on the number of correct responses within a time limit.

The Toolbox Picture Sequence Memory Test measures episodic memory and visuospatial sequencing (Bauer et al., 2013; Dikmen et al., 2014). Children are shown 15 pictures of activities or events and asked to reproduce the presentation order.

The Toolbox Oral Reading Recognition Task measures reading abilities by asking children to pronounce a series of written letters and words (Gershon et al., 2014).

Matrix Reasoning

The matrix reasoning subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Test for Children-V (WISC-V; Wechsler, 2014) measures fluid and spatial reasoning, perceptual organization, visual attention, and sequencing. On each trial, children are shown an array of visual stimuli, and are asked to select one of four stimuli that best completes the pattern. The task continues until a child makes three consecutive errors or completes 32 trials. Performance is measured by converting the number of total correct items to a standard score (Luciana et al., 2018).

Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test

The Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) measures learning and memory. During the test, children hear a list of 15 unrelated words five times. Each time they hear the list, they are asked to recall as many words as possible. After these five learning trials, children hear a distractor list and are again asked to recall as many words as they can. Recall of the initial list is assessed immediately after the distractor list and again 30 min later (Van Den Burg and Kingma, 1999; Luciana et al., 2018). Here, we measure performance as the number of correctly recalled words on these immediate and delayed memory assessments (i.e., RAVLT trials vi and vii).

Intertemporal cash choice task

The Intertemporal Cash Choice Task (Wulfert et al., 2002) assesses children’s delay of gratification, motivation, and impulsivity (Luciana et al., 2018). Children are asked, “Let’s pretend a kind person wanted to give you some money. Would you rather have $75 in 3 days or $115 in 3 months?” Smaller-sooner reward choices were coded with a “1,” larger-later reward choices were coded with a “2,” and infrequent “don’t know” responses were excluded from analysis. Positive correlations between cash choice and performance on another task indicate that children who performed better on the other task were more likely to choose the larger-later option, whereas negative correlations indicate that children who performed worse on the other task were more likely to choose the larger-later option.

Little Man Task

The Little Man Task (Acker and Acker, 1982) measures aspects of visuospatial processing including mental rotation. During this task, children see a cartoon of a man holding a briefcase in his left or right hand appear on a computer screen. The man can be right side up or upside down and can appear facing the child or with his back turned. Children are asked to indicate whether the man’s briefcase is in his left or right hand via button press. The task includes practice trials and 32 assessment trials. Performance is measured with efficiency (percent accuracy divided by mean correct-trial response time; Luciana et al., 2018).

Emotional n-back Task

The in-scanner emotional n-back (EN-back) task engages processes related to memory and emotion regulation (Barch et al., 2013; Casey, 2018). During the task, children perform 0-back (low memory load) and 2-back (high memory load) task blocks with four types of stimuli: happy, fearful, and neutral face photographs (Tottenham et al., 2009; Conley et al., 2018) and place photographs. Data are collected during two ;5-min fMRI runs each with four 0-back and 2-back blocks each. Runs included 362 whole-brain volumes after discarded acquisitions. At the start of each 0-back block, children are shown a target stimulus and asked to press a button corresponding to “match” when they see an identical picture and a button corresponding to “no match” when they see a different picture. During 2-back blocks, children are asked to press match when they see a picture identical to the one they saw two trials back. Performance is quantified as percent accuracy on 0-back and 2-back blocks.

Recognition Memory Task

After scanning, memory for EN-back task stimuli is assessed with a recognition memory test (Barch et al., 2013; Casey, 2018). During this test, children are presented with 48 EN-back stimuli and 48 novel stimuli (i.e., 12 old and new happy, fearful, and neutral face photographs and 12 old and new places), and are asked to rate whether each picture is “old” or “new.” Performance is assessed with sensitivity (d9) averaged across stimulus types.

Stop-signal Task

The in-scanner stop-signal task (SST; Logan, 1994) is designed to measure impulsivity and impulse control (Casey, 2018). SST data are collected during two ;6-min fMRI runs (437 volumes after discarded acquisitions) each with 180 trials each. On each trial, children see an arrow pointing to the left or to the right of the screen (the go signal). They are instructed to indicate the direction of the arrow with a button press as quickly and accurately as possible, except when an upright arrow (the stop signal) appears on the screen (16.67% of trials). The time between go and stop signal onset, the stop-signal delay, is staircased so that each child achieves ;50% accuracy on stop trials. Performance is measured with stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), or the mean stop-signal delay subtracted from the mean reaction time on correct go trials. For consistency with other behavioral measures, SSRTs were reverse scored (multiplied by –1) so that higher scores correspond to better performance.

Monetary Incentive Delay Task

The in-scanner MID task (Knutson et al., 2000; Yau et al., 2012) measures aspects of reward processing, including anticipation and receipt of rewards and losses and motivation to earn rewards and avoid losses (Casey, 2018). Data are collected during two ~5.5-min, 50-trial fMRI runs (403 volumes per run after discarded acquisitions). Trials begin with a cue indicating whether the child can win $0.20 or $5, lose $0.20 or $5, or earn $0. After 1500–4000 ms, a target appears for 150–500 ms. Target timing is staircased such that each child achieves ~60% accuracy. Children must respond during the target presentation to achieve the indicated trial outcome. Trials are followed by feedback indicating the outcome.

Response time and accuracy are not used as MID performance measures because target timing was individualized. Overall task performance is instead summarized as the average amount of money earned during both runs. This metric is correlated with mean accuracy (rs = 0.68, p , 0.001) and correct-trial RT (r = –0.29, p , 0.001), but may capture additional variance in reward-related behavior. For example, two children who achieve 60% accuracy could earn different amounts of money if one preferentially responds to win large rewards and avoid large losses, perhaps due to greater reward motivation.

Relationships Between Behavioral Measures

To characterize associations between working memory and other cognitive abilities, we first cross-correlated all 17 behavioral measures using data from all children meeting inclusion criteria (n = 11,537). Although normality was not evaluated with formal tests, which reject the null hypothesis for near-normal distributions in large samples (Ghasemi and Zahediasl, 2012), rank correlation was applied because visual inspection indicated that behavioral measures were not normally distributed (Fig. 1). To establish whether relationships were robust to potential confounds such as age, sex, missing data, outliers, and statistical dependence introduced by family structure, data collection method, and site, we subsequently replicated this analysis using:

(1) Data from one child per family, based on self-report (n = 9,750). For families with multiple children in the 11,537-participant cohort, the child whose randomly assigned NDAR Global Unique Identifier (GUID) came first in alphabetical order was included in this sample. (2) Data from all children meeting inclusion criteria using Spearman partial correlation to control for age and sex. (3) Data values within 2.5 SDs of the group mean (Table 1). (4) Data from children with no missing values (i.e., complete cases; n = 7,504). (For this analysis, “don’t know” responses on the cash choice task were counted as missing values.) (5) Data from children who completed the emotional n-back task, SST, and MID task during MRI data collection rather than on a laptop outside the scanner (n = 9,452). (Of the children whose data were analyzed here, 1,102 did not complete any task in the scanner or did not have this information available, 399 completed one task in the scanner, 584 completed two tasks in the scanner, and 9,452 completed all three tasks in the scanner.) (6) Data from children without performance flags on the emotional n-back task, SST, and MID task (n = 6,441). Performance flags, available in the curated ABCD data, were assigned based on the following criteria: <60% 0-back or 2-back accuracy on the emotional n-back task; <150 go trials, <60% go trial accuracy, >30% incorrect go trial percentage, >30% late go trial percentage, >30% “no response” go trials, <30 stop trials, or <20% or >80% stop trial accuracy on the SST; less than three positive and negative feedback events for large reward, small reward, large loss, small loss, or no stakes trials on the MID task. (7) Data from a conservative subsample excluding outlier values, incomplete cases, children who completed neuroimaging tasks outside of the scanner, children with neuroimaging task performance flags, and related children. Age and sex were controlled with partial correlation (n = 4,393). (8) Data from each of the 21 data collection sites with more than 100 participants separately (n = 328–988; mean n = 524.4; SD = 192.1; 22nd site with 36 children excluded from this analysis).

Because of the presence of missing data, relationships between behavioral measures were evaluated with pairwise correlations rather than with data reduction techniques such as principal component analysis (PCA), which do not typically allow for missing data. 34.96% of children were missing at least one performance measure, and neuroimaging task performance data were missing in 20.14% of the sample on average (Table 1; note that recovery of missing data are ongoing). Although Bayesian probabilistic PCA can account for missing data as well as the nesting of participants in families and data collection sites (Thompson et al., 2019), this approach assumes that missing data occur randomly, independent of other sample features (Oba et al., 2003). This assumption is violated in the current sample, as, for example, children with better working memory function are less likely to be missing other behavioral measures (Spearman correlation between list sorting performance and number of missing performance measures = –0.09, p , 2.2 1016). [Of note, Thompson et al. (2019) performed Bayesian probabilistic PCA on a subset of the behavioral measures analyzed here, specifically those collected outside the scanner. A smaller percentage of these data are missing from curated release 2.0.1 (,3% per measure; Table 1), reducing issues associated with missingness.]

Neuroimaging data collection

ABCD scan sessions included a localizer and acquisition of a high-resolution anatomic scan, two runs of resting state fMRI, diffusion weighted images, 3D T2-weighted spin echo images, two more runs of resting state fMRI, and task-based fMRI. [Sites with Siemens scanners used framewise integrated real-time MRI monitoring (FIRMM; Dosenbach et al., 2017) to monitor children’s head motion during data collection; scan operators at these sites may have stopped resting-state data collection after three runs if 12.5 min of low-motion resting-state data had been collected.] Image acquisition order was fixed, but fMRI task order was randomized across participants (Casey, 2018). Data were collected on Siemens Prisma, Phillips and GE 750 3T scanners, with detailed acquisition parameters reported in previous work (Casey, 2018; Hagler et al., 2019). Functional images were collected using a multiband gradient EPI sequence with the following parameters: TR = 800 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 52°, 60 slices acquired in the axial plane, voxel size = 2.4 mm3 , multiband slice acceleration factor = 6.

Image preprocessing

Task-based data were processed by the ABCD Study Data Analysis and Informatics Center (DAIC) using approaches described in detail in Hagler et al. (2019). Preprocessing steps included motion correction with 3dvolreg in AFNI, B0-distortion (i.e., field-map) correction, gradient nonlinearity distortion correction, and resampling scans into alignment with cubic interpolation using a mid-session scan as the reference. Registration between T2-weighted spin echo scans, field maps, and T1- weighted structural images was performed using mutual information. Functional images were aligned to T1-weighted images using rigid-body transformation (Hagler et al., 2019). The equivalent of 16 volumes was removed from the start of each run. For Siemens and Philips scanners, eight volumes were removed because the first eight volumes were used as the multiband reference scans. For GE scanners running DV25 software, five volumes were removed because the first 12 volumes were used as the multiband reference. The images were then combined into a single volume and saved as the initial TR (leaving a total of five frames to be discarded). For GE scanners running DV26 software, 16 volumes were removed. Relationships between fMRI activity and working memory function After preprocessing, voxel-wise time series data were normalized within run. Task-related activity estimates were generated for each child using general linear models (GLMs) with 3dDeconvolve in AFNI (Hagler et al., 2019). GLMs included nuisance regressors accounting for baseline and quadratic trends as well as motion estimates and their derivatives, temporally filtered to attenuate 0.31- to 0.43-Hz signals related to respiration (Fair et al., 2020). Volumes with framewise displacement values .0.9 mm were censored (Siegel et al., 2014).

In addition to fixation, the emotional n-back task GLM included predictors for happy, fearful, and neutral face as well as place stimuli in the 0-back and 2-back conditions. Task blocks (;24 s) were modeled as square waves convolved with a two-parameter gamma basis function (Hagler et al., 2019). The SST model included predictors for correct and incorrect stop and go trials, modeled as instantaneous. The MID model included small and large reward and loss cues and feedback and no stakes cues, modeled as instantaneous (Hagler et al., 2019). GLM beta coefficients for cortical gray matter voxels were sampled into surface space. [This step differs from the processing pipeline described in Hagler et al. (2019), in which preprocessed data were sampled onto the cortical surface, but does not affect the beta values.]

To characterize relationships between out-of-scanner List Sorting Working Memory performance and fMRI activation in response to a working memory challenge, we first estimated memory-related activity with a linear contrast of 2-back versus 0-back emotional n-back task blocks (6,965 datasets available; n = 3,750 after exclusion). Subject-specific beta weights were entered into a multiple regression model, including list sorting performance as a predictor, with FSL’s PALM software (Winkler et al., 2014). Covariates were also included in the model: age and sex (to account for effects present in the uncorrected list sorting standard scores; Luciana et al., 2018), scanner [to account for magnetrelated differences between the 26 scanners as well as effects of participant population (e.g., family income, education, race and ethnicity)], fluid intelligence (to account for non-specific effects of cognitive function), and mean frame-to-frame head motion during the emotional nback task. Head motion was measured with tfmri_nback_all_beta_mm from curated data release 2.0.1 sheet nback_bwroi02.txt. Degrees of freedom (tfmri_nback_all_beta_dof) and number of frames with a framewise displacement of ,0.2 mm (tfmri_nback_ab_subthnvols) were not included as covariates because they were highly correlated with frameto-frame motion (r = –0.96 and –0.95, respectively). Nonparametric significance was assessed with permutation testing using PALM’s tail approximation acceleration method (Winkler et al., 2016). For each contrast, 1,000 permutations were run, a generalized Pareto distribution (GPD) was fit to the tail of the resulting null distribution, and p values below 0.10 were computed from the GPD. Regression coefficients surviving a family-wise error-corrected p value threshold of 0.05 were considered significant.

To assess the specificity of relationships between list sorting scores and memory-related fMRI activity, we repeated this analysis using contrast maps related to three other processes: emotion processing, inhibitory control, and reward processing. Subject-specific beta coefficient maps reflecting emotion processing-related activity were computed by contrasting emotional versus neutral face stimulus blocks on the emotional n-back task (6,965 datasets available; n = 3,750 after exclusion). Beta coefficient maps reflecting inhibitory control-related activity were computed by contrasting successful versus unsuccessful stop trials on the SST (6,906 datasets available; n = 4,316 after exclusion). Beta coefficient maps reflecting activity related to reward sensitivity were computed by contrasting successful versus unsuccessful reward trials (i.e., volumes corresponding to positive vs negative feedback) on the MID task (6,984 datasets available; n = 4,277 after exclusion). Multiple regression models including working memory performance, age, sex, scanner, fluid intelligence, and mean frame-to-frame head motion during the relevant task runs were applied to predict subject-specific beta weights.

Neuroimaging Data Exclusion

Neuroimaging data from all Philips scanners (~13% of the sample) were excluded from analysis due to an error in curated ABCD data preprocessing. Neuroimaging data from children with poor structural scan quality, determined with curated data release 2.0.1 sheet freesqc01.txt, were also excluded from analysis. Participants with a score of zero for fsqc_qc and/or a score greater than one for fsqc_qu_motion, fsqc_qu_pialover, fsqc_qu_wmunder, or fsqc_qu_inhomogeneity were excluded. For each task contrast, participants with fewer than 550 degrees of freedom in preprocessed, concatenated fMRI time series, missing grayordinate (i.e., gray-matter vertex or voxel) values, and/or or extreme values (.3 SDs from the group mean) for the mean or SDs of beta weights over all grayordinates were also excluded. All fMRI analyses were performed using data from only one child per family to avoid confounds introduced by family structure. We elected to take this conservative approach rather than attempt to control for family structure with multilevel block permutation (Winkler et al., 2015) given the complexity of possible familial relationships (e.g., triplets, monozygotic and dizygotic twins, full siblings, half siblings, cousins, etc.).

Results

Working Memory Performance in Childhood

Working memory function, operationalized with NIH Toolbox List Sorting Working Memory Test performance, approximated the normative population mean (uncorrected standard score mean = 96.7, SD = 12.0, range = 36–136; normative mean = 100, SD = 15). Working memory was positively correlated with age (rs = 0.148, p , 2.2 1016) and differed by sex, albeit with a negligible effect size (female mean = 96.3; male mean = 97.2; Welch t(11317) = –3.99; p = 6.8 105 ; Cohen’s d = –0.075). Performance on all other neurocognitive measures in the ABCD task battery, which assess short term memory, fluid intelligence, visuospatial attention, reading and language skills, cognitive control, processing speed, flexible thinking, learning, delay of gratification, emotion regulation, impulsivity, and reward processing, is visualized in Figure 1.

Behavioral Signatures of Working Memory

Although a rich literature in cognitive psychology describes relationships between working memory and cognitive and attentional processes in adulthood, how these associations emerge in development is less well understood. Thus, a primary goal of the current work is to characterize these associations in childhood to understand how they change over time.

To relate working memory to cognitive and attentional abilities, we computed pairwise Spearman correlations between performance scores on all tasks included in the dataset (i.e., all behavioral measures visualized in Fig. 1). Correlation coefficients are reported without corresponding p values because effect sizes as small as r 2 = 3.34 104 are significant at p , 0.05 in a sample of 11,537, and statistical dependence introduced by family relatedness, site effects, and the inclusion of multiple performance measures per test precludes parametric r-to-p conversion. Furthermore, the goal of this analysis is to establish a pattern of behavioral relationships rather than to evaluate the statistical significance of particular associations.

Across individuals, working memory was most strongly related to language skills measured with the NIH Toolbox Picture Vocabulary and Oral Reading Recognition tests (rs values = 0.40); memory-related performance on the emotional n-back task, Picture Sequence Memory Test, and RAVLT; and fluid intelligence measured with matrix reasoning (rs = 0.35; Figs. 2, 3).

Correlations between list sorting performance and performance on other memory tests also revealed relationships between different aspects of memory. The emotional n-back task, collected during fMRI, measured performance during high memory load (2-back) and low memory load (0-back) task blocks. During 2-back blocks, children were asked to indicate when they saw a picture identical to the one they saw two trials back. During 0-back blocks, children were shown a target picture and instructed to indicate when they saw a matching image. Working memory was more strongly related to 2-back than to 0-back accuracy (rs = 0.36 vs 0.32; Steiger’s z = 5.36, p , 0.0001), indicating that, as predicted, working memory ability is reflected to a greater degree by performance on high-load versus lowload n-back blocks. [Interestingly, 2- back and 0-back accuracy scores were highly correlated (rs = 0.62), suggesting that common processes such as working memory, attention, and motivation contribute to performance on both tasks.] Recognition memory for emotional n-back stimuli (happy, fearful, and neutral face photographs and place photographs) was tested after fMRI data collection. Although working memory was less highly correlated with recognition memory than with performance on visual attention tasks, including the Flanker task (rs = 0.21 vs 0.25; Steiger’s z = 4.01, p = 0.0001), this may reflect low overall memory for specific stimuli, especially face photographs, at this age (Casey, 2018). Finally, the RAVLT assessed immediate recall of a word list as well as recall after a 30-min delay, and the NIH Toolbox Picture Sequence Memory Test measured episodic memory and visuospatial sequencing. Working memory was similarly related to immediate and delayed recall on the RAVLT and performance on the Picture Sequence Memory Test (rs values = 0.3389, 0.3372, and 0.3428, respectively).

Of note, correlations between list sorting performance and other behavioral measures are influenced by both the similarity of the constructs they measure and the reliability of the measures themselves. Previous work reports good to excellent reliability for all NIH Toolbox tasks [test-retest intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) = 0.61–0.90; Dikmen et al., 2014; Gershon et al., 2014; Tulsky et al., 2014; Zelazo et al., 2014; Carlozzi et al., 2015], the WISC-V matrix reasoning test (test-retest r = 0.78; Luciana et al., 2018), and the RAVLT (test-retest r = ;0.60–0.70; Van Den Burg and Kingma, 1999; Luciana et al., 2018). In the current sample, ICC, calculated by comparing performance during participants’ first and second fMRI runs, is 0.64 for the 0-back task and 0.68 for the 2-back task. However, the reliability of MID earnings is poor (ICC = 0.27). Thus, the low correlation between list sorting performance and measures including MID earnings may in part reflect low measure reliability. (Test-retest reliability estimates for the Intertemporal Cash Choice Task, Little Man Task, and post-scan n-back stimuli recognition memory test are not available because these tests were only completed once and reliability estimates have not been published elsewhere; testretest reliability was not calculated for the SST because run-specific stop-signal delay values were not available.)

Behavioral Relationships Replicate Across Independent Data Releases

To assess the reproducibility of behavioral relationships, we cross-correlated all performance measures using data from curated ABCD releases 1.1 (n = 4,397) and 2.0.1 (n = 7,140 new individuals) separately. The pattern of relationships was stable across releases (spatial rs = 0.982), suggesting that the observed pattern is generalizable rather than idiosyncratic to a particular sample. Behavioral relationships are not influenced by family structure The full 11,537-child cohort includes 3,532 related children from 9,750 unique families (based on self-report). Because relatedness affects the independence of behavioral measures and could have affected relationships between them, we replicated correlations between cognitive and attentional abilities in a subset of data from only one child per family. The pattern of behavioral relationships in this unrelated subsample was nearly identical to that observed in the full sample: the Spearman spatial correlation between the two samples’ vectorized behavioral cross-correlation matrices was 0.999.

Given the importance of accounting for family structure in big data samples, we next characterized effects of relatedness on behavioral relationships with an additional analysis. First, we computed the absolute difference between all 136 pairwise behavioral correlations in the full sample (n = 11,537) and the unrelated subsample (n = 9,750 after excluding 1,787 related children). Next, we randomly excluded 1,787 children from the full sample, re-calculated pairwise behavioral relationships, and recorded the difference between the full-sample correlations and these random subsample correlations. We repeated this process 1,000 times to generate a null distribution of correlation coefficient differences for each pair of behavioral measures. Nonparametric p values were generated by comparing each true correlation difference, |r(i,j)full sample – r(i,j)unrelated subsample|, to its corresponding null distribution. We used this conservative sub-sampling approach rather controlling for family relatedness with linear mixed models because of the range of possible familial relationships and the fact that relatedness may be inaccurately captured with self-report measures.

Using the subsampling approach, we found that none of the 136 pairwise behavioral relationships differed between the full sample and the unrelated subsample more than they differed between the full sample and the random subsamples (all . Bonferroni-corrected p = 0.05/136 = 3.68 104 ). Therefore, excluding family members from the sample did not disproportionately affect pairwise behavioral relationships, and including related participants does not bias the current results.

Behavioral Relationships are Robust to Age, Sex, Outliers, and Missing Data

Control analyses confirmed that behavioral relationships were robust to other potential confounds. Specifically, the overall pattern of relationships was consistent after controlling for age and sex with partial correlation (n = 11,537; rs = 0.995), excluding outlier values (.2.5 SDs from the group mean; n = 11,537; rs = 0.995), excluding children with any missing behavioral scores (n = 7,504; rs = 0.997), excluding children who completed any neuroimaging task (i.e., the emotional n-back task, SST, or MID task) on a laptop outside the scanner (n = 9,452; rs = 0.997), and excluding children with neuroimaging task performance flags provided by the ABCD Study (n = 6,441; rs = 0.982). The overall pattern of relationships was replicated to a lesser degree in a conservative subsample of children excluding relatives, outlier values, incomplete cases, children who completed neuroimaging tasks outside the scanner, and children with neuroimaging task performance flags, and controlling for age and sex (n = 4,393; rs = 0.973). Finally, associations between behavioral measures were similar across data collection sites despite differences in target sociodemographics (Garavan et al., 2018). Similarity between site-specific behavioral cross-correlation patterns ranged from rs = 0.80 to rs = 0.94 (mean rs = 0.86, SD = 0.03).

Neural Signatures of Working Memory

To identify a vertex-wise map of associations with working memory, we related out-of-scanner List Sorting Working Memory performance to fMRI activation in response to a working memory challenge. Working memory was significantly related to 2-back versus 0-back (i.e., high vs low memory load) activation in regions of frontal and parietal cortex including bilateral intraparietal sulci, dorsal premotor cortex/frontal eye fields, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex extending into the presupplementary motor area, and precuneus (Fig. 4). In line with previous work highlighting the importance of frontoparietal regions for working memory in development (Klingberg et al., 2002; Satterthwaite et al., 2013; Klingberg and Darki, 2015), children with better outof-scanner working memory performance showed increased activity during high relative to low memory load task blocks in this distributed set of regions that overlap with frontoparietal and dorsal attention networks (Power et al., 2011; Yeo et al., 2011; Fig. 5).

Relationships Between Working Memory and fMRI Activity are Domain Specific

Are patterns of fMRI activation that track individual differences in working memory driven by general task demands, or are they driven by working memory engagement per se? We performed two analyses to disentangle these alternatives. First, we examined the association between individual differences in working memory performance and activation revealed by a contrast emotional of versus neutral face blocks in the n-back task. Although these emotion-related activation patterns were measured during a working memory task, the contrast of these two types of face blocks does not reflect a working memory challenge. Therefore, significant relationships between these patterns and working memory would suggest that neural signatures of working memory are domain general rather than domain specific. Second, we examined the relationships between individual differences in working memory and activation patterns reflecting distinct cognitive processes in distinct task contexts: inhibitory control during an SST and reward processing during a MID task.

Results revealed that working memory was not significantly associated with emotion-related activation during the emotional nback task, inhibitory control-related activation during the SST, or reward-related activation during the MID task (Fig. 4). Although we did not compare regression coefficients across conditions because participant samples were overlapping but not identical, more participants and time points were available for the SST and MID than for the emotional n-back task. Furthermore, within-subject reliability of fMRI task activations (characterized by comparing beta weights in regions of the Desikan cortical brain atlas from participants’ first and second task runs, as included in curated data release 2.0.1) was numerically highest for the MID task contrast. Thus, the presence of significant effects for the working memory contrast, but not the inhibitory control or reward processing contrasts, is not attributable to sample size, amount of data per individual, or increased within-subject fMRI activation reliability for the working memory contrast, and results suggest that frontoparietal activity is a domain-specific rather than a domain-general signature of working memory.

Memory-related Frontoparietal Activity Reflects In-scanner and Out-of-scanner Working Memory Performance

One potential explanation of the current results is that in-scanner emotional n-back performance, a state-like measure of working memory and task engagement rather than a measure of individual differences in working memory per se, drives the selective relationship between working memory and 2-back versus 0-back frontoparietal activation. To evaluate this possibility, we replicated the analysis relating out-of-scanner working memory performance to 2-back versus 0-back activation with age, sex, scanner, fluid intelligence, mean frame-to-frame head motion during the n-back task, and 0-back and 2-back accuracy included in the model as covariates. Results revealed significant clusters in frontoparietal regions (Fig. 6), demonstrating that memoryrelated activation reflects both in-scanner and out-of-scanner working memory performance.

As an exploratory analysis, we next related 2-back versus 0-back activity to behavior with a multiple regression model that included all 17 behavioral measures reported in Figure 1 as well as age, sex, scanner, and mean frame-to-frame head motion during the n-back task. As expected, n-back activity predicted inscanner 2-back and 0-back accuracy, even when controlling for performance on all out-of-scanner cognitive tasks. In addition, significant (albeit qualitatively weaker) relationships were observed between 2-back versus 0-back activity and performance on the list sorting task and the out-of-scanner measures with which it was most highly correlated, the NIH Toolbox Picture Vocabulary and Oral Reading Recognition tasks and the WISCV matrix reasoning test. Activity was also significantly related to performance on the Little Man Task. Together, these results suggest that frontoparietal activity reflects both in-scanner and outof-scanner measures of working memory as well as behavioral performance on statistically related measures. Given that a limitation of this exploratory analysis is strong correlation among predictor variables, future work relating independent measures of cognitive and attentional processes to frontoparietal activity observed in multiple task contexts can inform the degree to which working memory-related activity specifically reflects working memory behavioral performance.

Neuroimaging findings replicate across independent data releases

To assess the reproducibility of observed brain–behavior relationships, we repeated fMRI analyses using data from participants first included in ABCD releases 1.0 and 2.0.1 separately. Consistent with the full-sample findings, List Sorting Working Memory Task performance was significantly related to 2-back versus 0-back activity in frontoparietal regions in both subsamples. List sorting performance was not significantly related to emotional versus neutral face, successful versus unsuccessful stop, or successful versus unsuccessful reward activity in either subsample (Fig. 7). When controlling for in-scanner n-back accuracy, the pattern of results was qualitatively similar, but significant relationships were observed in release-2 data only (release 1 n = 1,412; release 2 n = 2,338; Fig. 6).

Discussion

Working memory is a foundational cognitive function that changes over development and varies across individuals. Here, we characterize relationships between working memory, cognitive and attentional processes, and task-related brain activity in childhood using behavioral and fMRI data from the largest developmental neuroimaging sample to date. Behavioral analyses demonstrate that children with stronger working memory abilities perform better on a range of cognitive tasks, and revealed close relationships between working memory, performance on other memory tasks, language abilities, and fluid intelligence. fMRI analyses of emotional n-back task, SST, and MID task data provide evidence that frontoparietal activation in response to an explicit working memory challenge, but not in response to general task demands, is a reliable marker of working memory ability. Finally, a control analysis suggests that memory-related frontoparietal activity reflects individual differences in working memory above and beyond ongoing task performance.

Positive associations between working memory, language abilities, and fluid intelligence replicate previous work on the structure of cognition in children and adults (Engle et al., 1999; Gathercole, 1999; Thompson et al., 2019). As expected, children with stronger working memory performance (measured with the List Sorting Working Memory Test) also showed better performance on tests of episodic memory (Picture Sequence Memory), short-term memory (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning), and lowload and high-load working memory (emotional n-back 0- and 2-back conditions, respectively). Correlations between these measures in the full sample of 11,537 children ranged from 0.32 to 0.36, suggesting that they reflect both distinct and overlapping aspects of memory function. Somewhat surprisingly given established links between working memory and processing speed (Conway et al., 2002), working memory was less closely related to performance on the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test than to performance on every cognitive task except the SST, MID task, and Intertemporal Cash Choice task. Although the strength of the relationship between working memory and processing speed (rs = 0.20) is numerically similar to previous findings with the same tasks in 8- to 12-year-olds (r = 0.26; Carlozzi et al., 2015), individual differences in working memory were more strongly related to processes including executive attention and cognitive flexibility than to processing speed in the current cohort. Together, these results reveal relationships between working memory and cognitive and attentional processes in childhood.

This behavioral cross-correlation pattern was consistent after controlling for age and sex and excluding statistical outliers, incomplete cases, and neuroimaging task data collected outside the scanner. These behavioral patterns remained unchanged when measured in a subsample of the data that did not include relatives (i.e., only including one child per identified family). Thus, although it is important to account for these factors in large datasets such as the ABCD sample, the current results appear robust to effects of statistical dependence and outliers. Furthermore, the results are not biased by the inclusion of related children.

Neuroimaging results likewise align with previous work, providing evidence that frontoparietal activity reflects differences in working memory function during development (Klingberg et al., 2002; Satterthwaite et al., 2013). The narrow age range of the current sample, however, allowed us to disentangle individual differences from developmental changes, providing novel evidence that frontoparietal brain function underlies variability in working memory both within and across individuals. Furthermore, assessing relationships between working memory and fMRI activity related to memory, emotion processing, inhibitory control, and reward processing demonstrated that frontoparietal activation is a domain-specific rather than a task-general neural signature of working memory. Accounting for in-scanner emotional n-back performance, which reflects individual differences in working memory and attentional processes as well as transient attentional state, revealed relationships between out-of-scanner working memory performance and memory-related fMRI activation in regions of superior parietal and presupplementary motor cortex. Children with stronger working memory abilities, therefore, show increased frontoparietal activation during high relative to low memory load task blocks in part because they simply perform better on these tasks, but also because of individual differences in their ability to hold and manipulate information in mind.

The current results suggest that frontoparietal activation is a domain-specific neural signature of working memory in that individual differences in working memory are selectively reflected in 2-back versus 0-back frontoparietal activity. Results of an exploratory analysis also suggest some degree of specificity in the reverse direction: 2-back versus 0-back frontoparietal activity is uniquely related to both in-scanner and out-of-scanner working memory measures, as well as a subset of measures correlated with working memory task performance. Importantly however, frontoparietal activity does not only support working memory function but is also related to processes including attention and cognitive control (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Vincent et al., 2008; Spreng et al., 2010, 2013; Ptak, 2012). Recent work has emphasized the multifunctional nature of the frontoparietal network, proposing that it represents a domain-general “cognitive core” of the brain (Assem et al., 2020). Our results are not inconsistent with this conceptualization. Rather, they demonstrate that a high versus low memory load contrast reveals a frontoparietal activity signature of working memory, and leave open the possibility that an attention or cognitive control contrast could reveal a frontoparietal activity signature of attention or cognitive control. Future work that expands the collection of attention and control tasks and varies their cognitive demands will provide additional insights into the functional significance of overlapping and distinct patterns of frontoparietal activity across psychological tasks with development.

A neural signature of working memory based on task activation data complements a growing body of work identifying neuromarkers of individual differences from functional brain connectivity. In particular, patterns of task-based and restingstate functional connectivity, or statistical dependence between two brain regions’ activity time courses, have been used to predict individual differences in abilities including attention, fluid intelligence, and aspects of memory (Finn et al., 2015; Galeano Weber et al., 2017; Rosenberg et al., 2017; Lin et al., 2018; Rudolph et al., 2018; Yamashita et al., 2018; Avery et al., 2020). Recent work suggests that models based on task connectivity generally outperform those based on resting-state connectivity for predicting behavior, potentially because tasks engage circuits related to a process of interest to magnify individual differences in behaviorally relevant neural phenotypes, thereby improving predictions (Finn et al., 2017b; Greene et al., 2018; Rosenberg et al., 2018; Yoo et al., 2018). It is an open question, however, whether tasks selectively enhance the prediction of task-relevant behaviors. Here, motivated by previous work relating frontoparietal activation to developmental change in working memory (Klingberg et al., 2002; Kwon et al., 2002; Crone et al., 2006; Satterthwaite et al., 2013), we address this question with task activation rather than functional connectivity analyses. The current result, that frontoparietal activity indexes working memory only when working memory is explicitly taxed, suggests that task challenges may reveal neural signatures of task-relevant behaviors, and underscores the importance of multitask or multicondition data for elucidating state-specific and state-general biomarkers of behavior.

The goal of the current work was to characterize a brainbased biomarker and behavioral signature of working memory in childhood not just for the sake of understanding these relationships at a single point in time, but also for ultimately understanding their trajectories across development. Because the ABCD Study will follow children from age 9–10 to age 19–20, longitudinal work can provide new insights into associations between working memory, cognitive and attentional processes, and real-world outcomes across adolescence and young adulthood. Biennial MRI sessions, during which participants will complete the same emotional n-back task, SST, and MID task that they completed at ages 9 through 10, will also facilitate the discovery of changing neural signatures of abilities and behavior. For example, will there be changes in the distinct and overlapping brain activity patterns associated with working memory, inhibitory control, and reward processing with age? Will the domain specificity and domain generality of these signatures vary over time? Are there different developmental trajectories for frontoparietal organization of function across these processes? A fruitful way to frame the current findings is as a single point along a nonlinear trajectory rather than as a summary of working memory function in development as a whole.

Finally, as sample sizes in psychology and human neuroscience rapidly increase, it is important to note limitations of big data cohort-based approaches. First, behavioral and neuroimaging task batteries for these studies are determined by committee to address specific scientific goals. Although the resulting task sets often assess a range of mental processes, they may not be optimal for answering all questions. In the ABCD Study neuroimaging battery, for example, cognitive control demands and task difficulty are not equated across the emotional n-back task, SST, and MID task. Thus, the 2-back versus 0-back contrast may reflect processes such as cognitive control and attention that are not reflected in the three control contrasts. In addition, the individualized nature of the SST and MID task (which results in different timing parameters for different children) may obscure activation patterns related to individual differences in behavior. Furthermore, recent work highlights potential issues in the ABCD stop-signal task that may impact interpretation of the SST data (Bissett et al., 2020). Future work relating individual differences in working memory to fMRI activity reflecting cognitive control, attentional engagement, and other processes in contexts matched for task difficulty will further inform the domain specificity and generality of neural signatures of working memory. Second, large samples are not necessarily representative samples, and the ABCD cohort, while geographically, demographically, and socioeconomically diverse, should not be considered representative of the country or the world as a whole (Garavan et al., 2018). Looking ahead, future work relating cognitive and neural measures in weighted samples (LeWinn et al., 2017) can complement existing studies of single-site and multisite datasets. Third, just as the ABCD participant population may not represent youth as a whole, the structure of neurocognition in nine- and 10-year-olds likely does not reflect that of children at other ages. Longitudinal analyses of the ABCD cohort can inform changes in brain–behavior relationships across adolescence, and data collection efforts such as the Human Connectome Project (HCP) Development Study (Somerville et al., 2018) and HCP Aging Study (Bookheimer et al., 2019) can inform these associations in younger and older individuals. Finally, because even small effects can reach significance when samples are large, it is helpful to distinguish statistical from practical significance. Here, we focused on statistical significance as a proof-of-principle demonstration that memory-related frontoparietal activity tracks individual differences in working memory in childhood. Future work can evaluate practical or applied significance by testing whether models based on task activation patterns generalize to predict real-world outcomes including academic performance or changes in these outcomes over time.

Despite these limitations, the current results provide the most well powered characterization of relationships between working memory, cognitive and attentional processes, and task-based fMRI activation in development to date. Overall, they replicate established findings that children with stronger working memory function perform better on a variety of cognitive tasks, particularly those assessing other aspects of memory, language skills, and fluid intelligence. Furthermore, they provide evidence that frontoparietal network activation in response to an explicit working memory challenge is a robust and domain-specific marker of individual differences in working memory ability at age nine and ten. Together, these results inform understanding of the structure of neurocognition in childhood, and highlight the importance of evaluating brain–behavior relationships in multiple task contexts to demarcate the specificity and generality of neural signatures of abilities and behavior.

Link to Article

Abstract

Working memory function changes across development and varies across individuals. The patterns of behavior and brain function that track individual differences in working memory during human development, however, are not well understood. Here, we establish associations between working memory, other cognitive abilities, and functional MRI (fMRI) activation in data from over 11,500 9- to 10-year-old children (both sexes) enrolled in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, an ongoing longitudinal study in the United States. Behavioral analyses reveal robust relationships between working memory, short-term memory, language skills, and fluid intelligence. Analyses relating out-of-scanner working memory performance to memory-related fMRI activation in an emotional n-back task demonstrate that frontoparietal activity during a working memory challenge indexes working memory performance. This relationship is domain specific, such that fMRI activation related to emotion processing during the emotional n-back task, inhibitory control during a stop-signal task (SST), and reward processing during a monetary incentive delay (MID) task does not track memory abilities. Together, these results inform our understanding of individual differences in working memory in childhood and lay the groundwork for characterizing the ways in which they change across adolescence.

Neural and Behavioral Signatures of Working Memory in Childhood

Working memory, a cognitive process responsible for storing and manipulating information, varies across individuals and changes over development. This study investigates the relationships between working memory, other cognitive abilities, and brain activity in childhood using data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study.

Behavioral Findings

  • Children with stronger working memory performed better on tasks assessing memory, language abilities, and fluid intelligence.

  • Working memory was closely related to performance on episodic memory, short-term memory, and high-load working memory tasks.

  • Relationships between working memory and other cognitive abilities were consistent across different data subsets and statistical controls.

Neuroimaging Findings

  • Frontoparietal brain activity during a working memory challenge (2-back vs. 0-back emotional n-back task) was associated with individual differences in working memory.

  • This relationship was specific to working memory, as frontoparietal activity did not correlate with performance on tasks measuring emotion processing, inhibitory control, or reward processing.

  • Memory-related frontoparietal activity reflected both in-scanner task performance and out-of-scanner working memory measures.

Discussion

These findings suggest that frontoparietal activity during a working memory challenge is a reliable neural marker of working memory ability in childhood. The domain specificity of this signature indicates that frontoparietal activity reflects working memory function rather than general task demands.

The study also highlights the importance of using task-based fMRI to identify neural signatures of specific cognitive processes. By comparing brain activity during different task conditions, researchers can isolate neural patterns that are uniquely associated with the process of interest.

Future research can leverage the longitudinal nature of the ABCD Study to track changes in brain–behavior relationships across adolescence. Additionally, exploring the practical significance of these findings by examining their predictive value for real-world outcomes is an important next step.

Link to Article

Abstract

Working memory function changes across development and varies across individuals. The patterns of behavior and brain function that track individual differences in working memory during human development, however, are not well understood. Here, we establish associations between working memory, other cognitive abilities, and functional MRI (fMRI) activation in data from over 11,500 9- to 10-year-old children (both sexes) enrolled in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, an ongoing longitudinal study in the United States. Behavioral analyses reveal robust relationships between working memory, short-term memory, language skills, and fluid intelligence. Analyses relating out-of-scanner working memory performance to memory-related fMRI activation in an emotional n-back task demonstrate that frontoparietal activity during a working memory challenge indexes working memory performance. This relationship is domain specific, such that fMRI activation related to emotion processing during the emotional n-back task, inhibitory control during a stop-signal task (SST), and reward processing during a monetary incentive delay (MID) task does not track memory abilities. Together, these results inform our understanding of individual differences in working memory in childhood and lay the groundwork for characterizing the ways in which they change across adolescence.

Neural and Behavioral Signatures of Working Memory in Childhood

Introduction

Working memory, the ability to store and manipulate information, is crucial for many cognitive functions and varies among individuals. This study investigates the relationships between working memory, other cognitive abilities, and brain activity in childhood.

Behavioral Findings

  • Children with better working memory performed better on tasks involving memory, language, and fluid intelligence (the ability to solve novel problems).

  • Working memory was most strongly related to:

    • Language skills (vocabulary and reading)

    • Other memory tasks (remembering word lists and sequences)

    • Fluid intelligence (reasoning and problem-solving)

Neural Findings

  • Brain imaging (fMRI) revealed that children with better working memory showed increased activity in brain regions known as the frontoparietal network when performing a working memory task.

  • This activity pattern was specific to working memory and not observed during tasks involving emotion processing, inhibitory control, or reward processing.

  • The relationship between working memory and frontoparietal activity remained significant even after accounting for performance on the working memory task itself. This suggests that the activity reflects individual differences in working memory ability beyond simply how well children performed on the task.

Discussion

  • The findings support previous research showing that frontoparietal brain activity is associated with working memory in children.

  • The specificity of the activity pattern suggests that it is a reliable neural marker of working memory ability.

  • The study also highlights the importance of using tasks that specifically challenge working memory to identify its neural signatures.

Implications and Future Directions

  • The findings provide insights into the structure of cognition in childhood and how it may change over time.

  • Future research can investigate how these brain-behavior relationships develop across adolescence and into adulthood.

  • The neural signature of working memory may be useful for predicting real-world outcomes, such as academic performance.

Limitations

  • The study focused on a specific age range (9-10 years old), so the findings may not generalize to other ages.

  • The tasks used in the study may not have been optimal for assessing all aspects of working memory.

  • The large sample size may have led to the detection of small effects that may not be practically significant.

Link to Article

Abstract

Working memory function changes across development and varies across individuals. The patterns of behavior and brain function that track individual differences in working memory during human development, however, are not well understood. Here, we establish associations between working memory, other cognitive abilities, and functional MRI (fMRI) activation in data from over 11,500 9- to 10-year-old children (both sexes) enrolled in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, an ongoing longitudinal study in the United States. Behavioral analyses reveal robust relationships between working memory, short-term memory, language skills, and fluid intelligence. Analyses relating out-of-scanner working memory performance to memory-related fMRI activation in an emotional n-back task demonstrate that frontoparietal activity during a working memory challenge indexes working memory performance. This relationship is domain specific, such that fMRI activation related to emotion processing during the emotional n-back task, inhibitory control during a stop-signal task (SST), and reward processing during a monetary incentive delay (MID) task does not track memory abilities. Together, these results inform our understanding of individual differences in working memory in childhood and lay the groundwork for characterizing the ways in which they change across adolescence.

Working Memory: A Key Cognitive Ability in Childhood

Working memory is like your brain's scratchpad. It helps you hold and use information for a short time, like remembering a phone number or following instructions. This ability changes as we grow and varies from person to person.

How Working Memory Relates to Other Abilities

Researchers studied over 11,000 kids and found that those with better working memory also did better on other cognitive tasks, especially those involving:

  • Memory (like remembering a list of words or a sequence of pictures)

  • Language (like understanding and using words)

  • Problem-solving (like figuring out puzzles)

Brain Activity and Working Memory

The researchers also used brain scans (fMRI) to see how working memory is related to brain activity. They found that when kids were given a working memory challenge, certain areas of their brains became more active. These areas are called the frontoparietal network.

Key Finding: Kids with better working memory showed more activity in the frontoparietal network during the working memory challenge.

Is This Brain Activity Specific to Working Memory?

To find out, the researchers looked at brain activity during other tasks, like controlling their impulses or processing emotions. They found that the frontoparietal network was not as active during these tasks.

Conclusion: The frontoparietal network seems to be specifically involved in working memory.

Beyond the Brain Scan

Interestingly, the researchers found that the frontoparietal activity was related to how well the kids performed on the working memory task itself. This suggests that the brain activity reflects both how well the kids are doing on the task and their overall working memory ability.

Future Directions

This study provides a snapshot of working memory in childhood. As these kids grow older, researchers will continue to track their brain activity and cognitive abilities. This will help us understand how working memory changes over time and how it affects real-world outcomes like academic success.

Link to Article

Abstract

Working memory function changes across development and varies across individuals. The patterns of behavior and brain function that track individual differences in working memory during human development, however, are not well understood. Here, we establish associations between working memory, other cognitive abilities, and functional MRI (fMRI) activation in data from over 11,500 9- to 10-year-old children (both sexes) enrolled in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, an ongoing longitudinal study in the United States. Behavioral analyses reveal robust relationships between working memory, short-term memory, language skills, and fluid intelligence. Analyses relating out-of-scanner working memory performance to memory-related fMRI activation in an emotional n-back task demonstrate that frontoparietal activity during a working memory challenge indexes working memory performance. This relationship is domain specific, such that fMRI activation related to emotion processing during the emotional n-back task, inhibitory control during a stop-signal task (SST), and reward processing during a monetary incentive delay (MID) task does not track memory abilities. Together, these results inform our understanding of individual differences in working memory in childhood and lay the groundwork for characterizing the ways in which they change across adolescence.

Working Memory: How Does Your Brain Hold Information?

Have you ever wondered why some kids are better at remembering things than others? It's because of something called working memory. It's like a special storage space in your brain that helps you hold and use information for a short time.

Working Memory and Other Brain Skills

Scientists studied a lot of kids and found that those with better working memory also did better on other brain tasks, like:

  • Remembering lists of words and pictures

  • Understanding language and reading

  • Solving puzzles and making decisions

Your Brain's Working Memory Network

When your brain is working hard to remember something, special areas in your brain light up. These areas are called the frontoparietal network. Kids with better working memory show more activity in this network when they're trying to remember things.

It's Not Just About the Task

The cool thing is that this brain activity doesn't just show up when you're doing a memory test. It also shows up when you're doing other tasks that need your attention and focus. So, working memory is like a superpower that helps you with lots of different things.

Your Working Memory Superpower

Just like some kids are better at running or jumping, some kids have a better working memory. But don't worry, there are things you can do to improve your working memory, like playing memory games and practicing focusing on one thing at a time.

Link to Article

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Rosenberg, M. D., Martinez, S. A., Rapuano, K. M., Conley, M. I., Cohen, A. O., Cornejo, M. D., ... & Casey, B. J. (2020). Behavioral and neural signatures of working memory in childhood. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(26), 5090-5104. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2841-19.2020

    Highlights