Anticipatory reward dysfunction in alcohol dependence: An electroencephalography monetary incentive delay task study
Mica Komarnyckyj
Chris Retzler
Robert Whelan
Oliver Young
Elsa Fouragnan
SimpleOriginal

Summary

This 2023 study used EEG to examine how people in recovery from alcohol dependence process rewards.They showed weaker brain responses to reward cues and stronger signals during action prep, reflecting altered motivation and control.

2023

Anticipatory reward dysfunction in alcohol dependence: An electroencephalography monetary incentive delay task study

Keywords Event-related potentials; Alcohol dependence; Reward; EEG; Addiction; Computational psychiatry

Abstract

A wealth of functional magnetic resonance imaging monetary incentive delay task (MIDT) research has shown alcohol dependency is associated with a hypoactive striatal response during gain anticipation (gain > neutral) and loss anticipation (loss > neutral). Electroencephalography (EEG) holds clinical advantages over fMRI (high temporal resolution, low cost, portable) however its use to study reward processing in alcohol dependence is limited. We aimed to carry out the first EEG MIDT (eMIDT) study in alcohol dependence. 21 abstinent alcohol dependent individuals and 26 controls performed an MIDT while neural activity was recorded using 64-channel EEG. Trial averaged event-related potentials (ERPs) and single-trial machine learning discriminant analyses were applied to EEG data. Clinical variables related to severity of dependence were collected and relationships with ERP data explored. Alcohol dependent individuals, compared with healthy controls, had blunted cue-P3 amplitudes for gain and loss anticipation (interaction: p = 0.019); and elevated contingent negative variation amplitudes for all conditions (gain, loss, neutral)(main effect: p < 0.001) which was associated with increased alcohol consumption (p = 0.002). The machine learning analyses demonstrated alcohol dependent individuals had reduced ability to discriminate between loss and neutral cues between 328 – 350 ms (p = 0.040), 354 – 367 ms (p = 0.047) and 525 – 572 ms (p = 0.022). The eMIDT approach is demonstrated to be a low-cost, sensitive measure of dysfunctional anticipatory reward processing in alcohol dependence, which we propose is ideal for big data approaches to prognostic psychiatry and translation into clinical practice.

Introduction

Converging research suggests that disruption of the neurobiological networks responsible for reward processing increases vulnerability and supports the maintenance of addiction [1]. The functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) monetary incentive delay task (MIDT) (fMRI-MIDT) has been used extensively to investigate reward function in drug and alcohol dependence. Its strength is the ability to probe both appetitive and consummatory phases of reward processing without excessive demand upon executive function [2,3]. Supporting the ‘reward deficiency syndrome’ theory of addiction [4], the majority of fMRI-MIDT studies of drug and alcohol dependence report hypoactivation of key reward processing regions, including the ventral striatum, during both monetary gain and loss anticipation [3,[5], [6], [7], [8]]. Other fMRI-MIDT studies, however, did not replicate these findings, instead reporting comparable striatal sensivity during anticipatory reward processing in alcohol dependent partcipants and controls [9], [10], [11].

Despite the popularity of the fMRI-MIDT in addiction research, the temporal resolution of the fMRI blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal (in the range of seconds) is not optimal for studying reward-related processing that occurs in the sub-second range [12,13], possibly contributing to inconsistent findings within the fMRI-MIDT literature. In contrast to fMRI, the electroencephalography (EEG) signal is a more direct measure of neural activity, with a temporal resolution in the range of milliseconds, ideal for indexing the range of neural processes that occur during anticipatory and consummatory reward. Disadvantages of EEG include a relatively poor spatial resolution compared with fMRI and issues with detecting signal from deeper brain sources, meaning EEG is unable to precisely measure activity within subcortical reward regions [14]. However, combined EEG-fMRI research suggests EEG is able to probe reward function by detecting cortical downstream consequences of subcortical reward network activation [15]. An additional advantage of EEG is its low setup (£17,000 - £75,000) and running costs [16], whereas fMRI has prohibitive costs (∼£2.5 million investment, ∼£500/hour rental) which potentially limit study design and present a barrier for translation into clinical practice [17]. The cost effectiveness of EEG is more amenable to widespread clinical use, big data approaches to clinical risk/ treatment response prediction and other ambitious study designs (i.e., large longitudinal studies or neuropharmacological studies investigating multiple drug doses).

EEG versions of the MIDT (eMIDT) have been used successfully to study reward processing in healthy controls (HC), highlighting the cue-P3 event-related potential (ERP) to be an important neurophysiological marker of reward [18], [19], [20]. This broad positive wave typically arises between 250 – 600 ms post stimulus onset and is commonly broken down into early cue-P3a and late cue-P3b subcomponents within eMIDT literature [18,21,22]. The cue-P3a and cue-P3b are theorised to reflect stimulus-driven attentional mechanisms and memory/salience processing, respectively [18,23] and have been highlighted as potential clinical markers of alcohol dependence in a recent review article [24].

Research has shown the broad cue-P3 wave (250 – 500 ms) is potentially modulated by central dopamine systems [25]. eMIDT studies with HC demonstrated cue-P3 amplitudes are generally enhanced for monetary gain anticipation [18,20,21,26] and loss anticipation [18,20] and covary with fMRI ventral striatal MIDT response [15]. These findings reflect the ventral striatum BOLD response reported in fMRI-MIDT studies with HC [6], [7], [8] and taken together demonstrate the cue-P3 component reliably indexes appetitive neural processing within the reward network.

A further component of interest is the contingent negative variation (CNV), a slow wave related to anticipatory attention and preparation of motivated responses [27,28]. Although, to our knowledge, there are no previous eMIDT studies with substance use disorder or AD participants, our previous eMIDT study with young adult at-risk drinkers showed a trend towards hyperactive CNV salience response (monetary incentive > neutral cues) [21]. A further eMIDT study demonstrated an enhanced CNV salience response (monetary > verbal reward cues) in young adults who had their first drink of alcohol during puberty compared to those who first drank alcohol post-puberty [29].

Here, we carried out the first eMIDT study in alcohol dependence with the aim of investigating anticipatory reward processing with high temporal precision. In addition to traditional ERP analyses, we employed an exploratory whole-brain multivariate linear discriminant machine learning (LD-ML) approach [30,31]. By decoding the experimental conditions across the whole-brain and exploiting information across all electrodes and trials, this method avoids a priori electrode choice and achieves improved signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) [32]. Our prior research identified this LD-ML method to have greater sensitivity over traditional ERP methods for detecting reward dysfunction in young-adult at-risk drinkers [21]. We hypothesised AD participants, relative to HC, would exhibit 1) a hypoactive cue-P3 gain and loss anticipatory response [3,[5], [6], [7], [8],15], 2) a hyperactive CNV salience anticipatory response [21,29], and 3) the magnitude of cue-P3 and CNV would be associated with clinical symptoms of AD (including levels of alcohol consumption and obsessive-compulsive thoughts about alcohol).

Materials and methods

Participants

This study included 21 abstinent AD participants (13 male) (mean abstinence duration: 25 months, range: 1 – 130 months; mean age: 45.1 ± 9.3, range: 29 – 60 years) and 26 HC participants (17 male) (mean age: 40.7 ± 10.8, range: 24 – 60 years) (Table 1). We recorded EEG data from 50 participants but excluded 3 due to their failure to complete the task correctly (Supplementary Methods). Participants were recruited from the University of Huddersfield (UK) campus and local community via flyers, posters, email campaigns, word of mouth, and Facebook groups. AD participants were also recruited from alcohol services in Huddersfield (flyers/posters) and surrounding areas via a targeted Facebook advertising campaign.

Table 1. Demographic and clinical measures.

Table 1

All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, normal hearing and were able to read, comprehend and record information in English. AD participants met the DSM-V criteria for severe alcohol use disorder (i.e., score > 6 out of a total score of 11) in their most recent year of drinking alcohol prior to their current period of abstinence (DSM-V mean score: 10.76, score range: 9 - 11), had never met the DSM-V criteria for any other substance use disorder (except nicotine) [33] and were abstinent from alcohol for a minimum of 4 weeks prior to study participation (there was no upper limit for abstinence duration). HC participants had never met the DSM-V criteria for alcohol or drug dependence (except nicotine) [33] and had no first-degree relatives with a history of drug or alcohol dependence. Nicotine dependence was not part of the exclusion criteria for the study. A more detailed description of exclusion criteria is included within the Supplementary Methods.

The study was approved by the University of Huddersfield Department of Applied Sciences Ethics Committee (reference: SAS-SREIC 1.5.19–1).

Experimental sessions

Telephone screening was conducted to verify whether participants met basic eligibility criteria before they were invited to a face-to-face clinical screening which included: written informed consent; alcohol breathalyser and urine drug test (AllTEST™ 10 panel); Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.) [34] and DSM-V interview for drug or alcohol dependence; drug and alcohol timeline follow-back interview; Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI) [35]; Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) [36]; an online battery of questionnaires (hosted by Qualtrics) which collected demographic and clinical information (Table 1); and 30 practice trials of the eMIDT.

Participants found to be eligible during the clinical screening attended an EEG recording session during which they completed an alcohol breathalyser and urine drug test (AllTEST™ 10 panel), 30 practice trials of the eMIDT and the full eMIDT which consisted of 240 trials. Participants were reimbursed for travel costs and received £10 for attending the clinical screening and £11 - £14 for the EEG recording (depending on eMIDT performance). HC participants could select from Amazon/Sainsbury's vouchers for reimbursement, however, AD participants were given solely Amazon vouchers, since the visibility and accessibility of alcohol in supermarkets may have been problematic.

Experimental design

Participants performed an EEG version of the MID task (Fig. 1), adapted from that originally used by Knutson with fMRI [2]. A cue symbol was presented for 250 ms which informed participants of the trial type (gain, loss, neutral). A square containing an ascending arrow indicated the potential to win 50p (i.e., gain trials); a square containing a descending arrow indicated the potential to lose 50p (i.e., loss trials); and a square containing a horizontal line indicated there would be no financial outcome (i.e., neutral trials). A white fixation cross was then presented (random jitter of 2000 – 2500 ms) during which participants prepared their motor response for a square target which was presented immediately after.

Figure 1

Fig. 1. EEG monetary incentive delay task paradigm.

Note: Schematic representation of the eMIDT experimental paradigm. On each trial, one of three cue symbols was presented for 0.25 s indicating if participants could win or lose 50p, or if the trial would have no impact on earnings (i.e., neutral). Following a jittered delay of 2 – 2.5 s a square target was presented. Following a jittered delay of 1 – 1.2 s feedback was shown. A jittered inter-trial interval of 1 – 1.2 s was presented before the next trial began.

Participants were instructed to press the space bar as quickly as possible (not more than one time) when the target appeared. A staircase algorithm adapted the target duration to maintain an accuracy of ∼66% within each trial type. If accuracy rose above 66% and the participant hit the target, the duration was reduced by 16.67 ms. If accuracy was less than or equal to 66% and the participant missed the target, the duration was increased by 16.67 ms. On all other trials, the target duration remained unaltered. The algorithm ensured target duration did not fall below 150 ms or go above 350 ms. The starting point for the target duration was informed by the participant's reaction time (RT) on the 30 practice trials.

Following target response, a white fixation cross was presented (random jitter of 1000 – 1200 ms) and participants prepared to view their feedback (shown for 1000 ms). On gain and loss trials participants were shown a tick if they hit the target and a cross if they missed the target, leading to a total of four different outcomes: (1) gain hit trials, a tick was shown and monetary earnings increased by 50 pence; (2) gain miss trials, a cross was shown and monetary earnings stayed the same; (3) loss hit trials, a tick was shown and monetary earnings stayed the same; (4) loss miss, a cross was shown and monetary earnings decreased by 50 pence. Neutral trials had no impact on monetary earnings and no performance feedback was given, instead a horizontal line was shown in place of the tick and cross. Each participant completed 5 blocks of 48 trials each lasting ∼4.7 min. The task comprised of 80 win, 80 loss and 80 neutral trials and lasted ∼24 min (excluding breaks).

Experimental stimuli and setup

Our eMIDT task included 3 cue stimuli (Fig. 1) (170 × 141 pixels) which have been validated in prior fMRI-MID task research published by members of our research group [7,37]. Other symbols included a square target (90 × 90 pixels), tick feedback symbol (158 × 98 pixels), cross feedback symbol (157 × 82 pixels) and line feedback symbol (160 × 37 pixels). All symbols were presented in white on a black background. The task was programmed with Psychopy version 1.90.1 [38] (https://www.psychopy.org/ ). It was presented at a frame rate of 60 Hz, using a computer running on Windows 10 (64 bit, 3GB RAM, nVidia GeForce GT 710 graphics card), on a monitor with a resolution of 1920 × 1080 pixels. Participants sat 50 cm from the monitor and responded by pressing the space bar on a USB keypad with the index finger on their dominant hand.

EEG data acquisition and pre-processing

EEG data were recorded at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz from 64 Ag/AgCl active electrodes, using a 10–20 layout and online reference channel of FCz (ActiCap – Brain Vision, Brain Products, Germany). Channel impedances were kept below 25 kΩ. Pre-processing was performed in EEGlab Toolbox [39] running on Matlab R2019b. Raw data were first downsampled to 500 Hz and high pass filtered at 0.1 Hz to attenuate slow frequency noise. EEG segments where instructions/breaks occurred were removed to improve subsequent channel rejection and independent component analyses (ICA).

The Clean Rawdata EEGlab plug-in (version 2.0) (https://github.com/sccn/clean_rawdata ) was used to reject bad channels (including flat and correlated) which were subsequently interpolated. The CleanLine EEGlab plug-in (version 1.04) (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/cleanline ) was used to remove 50 Hz power noise. ICA was employed with principal component analysis enabled to reduce dimensions based on a new data rank, accounting for rank deficiency due to channel rejection and average referencing [40]. The Multiple Artefact Rejection Algorithm (http://github.com/irenne/MARA ) was used to ensure ICAs were only rejected if there was a high probability (>98%) they represented artefact. The data were low pass filtered at 40 Hz to attenuate high-frequency noise. Finally, the ERPlab plugin [41] (https://erpinfo.org/erplab ) was used to extract cue-locked epochs from the continuous data, with a 500 ms prestimulus and 2000 ms poststimulus period. All epochs were baseline corrected by subtracting the mean voltage of the prestimulus interval between −200 and 0 ms from each epoch, this was done separately for each channel. To conserve as much of the original EEG dataset as possible, epoch rejection was not conducted as part of the pre-processing pipeline. In line with our prior research [31,42], this maintained trial-by-trial variability which is important for the LD-ML analyses. Furthermore, it ensured the maximum number of trials were available for the ERP analyses (80 per condition) thus avoiding reduced statistical power which may result from rejecting bad data epochs prior to trial averaging to form ERPs [43]. However, individual subject ERPs were reviewed manually to ensure data cleaning methods had been effective.

Event related potential (ERP) analyses

Based on prior eMIDT literature, the cue-P3 was broken down into the cue-P3a and cue-P3b subcomponents, which were quantified over a parietal electrode group (P1, P2, POz, Pz) [21,22,44] and measured as the mean amplitude over 300 – 450 ms and 450 – 600 ms following cue onset, respectively. CNV was measured as the mean amplitude 1800 to 2000 ms post cue onset over a centro-frontal electrode group (C1, C2, FCz, Fz) [21,44].

Mixed model analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) were conducted on mean ERP amplitudes to evaluate group differences. Age, IQ and smoking status were included as covariates based on prior research which has shown these demographic variables can affect the neural response to reward and loss processing [45], [46], [47]. The 3 × 2 ANCOVA models used condition (i.e., gain, loss, neutral) as the within-subjects factor and group (i.e., HC group, AD group) as the between-subjects factor. These tests were performed separately for all ERPs (cue-P3a, cue-P3b, CNV).

ERP data entered into the ANCOVA models were checked using Mauchly's Test of Sphericity, if a violation occurred, a Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied. When ANCOVA tests revealed significant overall results, Tukey post-hoc tests were conducted to investigate which conditions differed significantly from one another. For all statistical tests we used a significance level of p < 0.05. R software (version 4.0.5) was used for all ERP statistical analyses.

Associations between ERP data and clinical severity

For the three time windows of interest (cue-P3a, cue-P3b, CNV), we explored associations between ERP data (ERP difference waves (µV): gain-minus-neutral, loss-minus-neutral; and ERP mean amplitude (µV): gain, loss) and clinical variables of lifetime alcohol consumption (alcohol units consumed per adult year of active drinking) and Obsessive Compulsive Drinking Scale (OCDS) score (based on an amended version of the OCDS which did not include questions relating to alcohol consumption, see Supplementary Methods) [48], yielding 24 two-way linear regression models (Supplementary Methods). The p.adjust function from The R Stats Package [49] was used to apply false discovery rate (FDR) correction for multiple comparisons (24 models). Only models in which ERP data or ERP data by group interaction term was significantly associated with a clinical variable following FDR correction (p < 0.05) are included in the results. Diagnostic plots were evaluated from all models and outliers removed (Cook's distance > 0.5). This resulted in the removal of a single data point from three regression models.

Within two-way linear regression models, the intercept/constant term refers to the estimated mean Y-value for the reference group, and model coefficients relate to expected changes in mean Y-value relative to this reference [49]. As the present study is focused on identifying reward processing abnormalities associated with alcohol dependency, the AD group was initially specified as the reference group. In models where the ERP data by group interaction term was found to be significant, the linear regression model was rerun specifying HC as the reference group to confirm how the nature of the association differed between groups. R software (version 4.0.5) was used for all regression analyses.

Machine learning analyses

We conducted multivariate LD-ML on the single-trial epoched EEG data [21,31]. For each participant, we conducted binary discriminations across the loss anticipation (loss-vs-neutral) and gain anticipation (gain-vs-neutral) dimensions of reward processing. We estimated a linear weighting contribution (i.e., spatial weighting vector w(τ)or spatial filter) for each EEG electrode across the whole brain that gave maximum discrimination between conditions of interest Eq. (1). The LD-ML analyses were conducted across temporally unique training windows, each with a fixed length of δ = 60 ms and centre times τ varying from −200 to 2000 ms after cue onset (increasing in increments of 10 ms). A regularised Fisher discriminant estimated w(τ)which provided maximum discrimination between electrode signals x(t)for two conditions (further details are provided in the Supplementary Methods).(1)yi(τ)=1N∑t=τ−N/2t=τ+N/2w(τ)Txi(t)

To evaluate discriminator performance at each training window, we calculated the area under a receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve (Az) using a leave-one-trial-out (LOTO) cross validation approach randomizing the labels for each trial. The randomisation procedure was repeated 100 times, producing a probability distribution for Az and allowing estimation of the Az which gave a significance level of p < 0.05.

We statistically compared group averaged discriminator performance, Az values, using SPM1d version 0.4.7 (open-source MATLAB software available from http://www.spm1d.org/ ), as demonstrated in our prior research [21]. SPM1d calculates a scalar trajectory output statistic, SPM{t}, for individual time nodes across a 1D continuum; where time nodes represented onset times τ for the multivariate discriminant analyses and the 1D continuum were pre-hypothesised time windows for components of interest (e.g., cue-P3 and CNV). The SPM{t} represents only the magnitude of differences between group Az values over the 1D continuum, therefore using this scalar trajectory alone will not give conclusive evidence to reject or accept the null hypotheses (i.e., no significant differences in group averaged Az values). Random field theory (RFT) was therefore used to define a critical threshold (α) that no greater than 5% (α < 0.05) of random data that is equally smoothed would be likely to cross. We therefore were able to reject the null hypotheses if SPM{t} exceeded the critical threshold at any time node [50].

Results

Behavioural responses

The mixed model ANCOVA for RT, 3 (condition) x 2 (group) (age, IQ and smoking status as covariates) showed a significant main effect of condition on RT (F(2, 84) = 34.84, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.453). Tukey's post-hoc tests revealed that RT for the neutral condition was significantly longer than RT for the gain (p < 0.001) and loss (p < 0.001) conditions (Table 2). Consistent with prior fMRI-MIDT literature with AD participants [6], [7], [8], we found no significant group effects or interactions for RT.

Table 2. Behavioural data.

Table 2

Effects of alcohol dependency on the cue-P3

For the cue-P3a we found a significant main effect of condition (F(2, 84) = 7.25, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.148) but no main effect of group (F(1, 42) = 0.04, p = 0.826, ηp2 = 0.001) or group by condition interaction (F(2, 84) = 0.01, p = 0.991, ηp2 < 0.001). Tukey's post-hoc tests revealed cue-P3a amplitude was significantly larger for the gain condition compared to the neutral condition (p = 0.004) (Fig. 2a and b).

Figure 2

Fig. 2. ERP results for the cue-P3a and cue-P3b time windows.Note: Average cue-P3 ERP components for the HC group (a) and AD group (b), computed over parietal electrodes [P1, P2, POz, Pz]. The first and second grey shaded bars depict the time windows for the cue-P3a and cue-P3b respectively. Scalp topographies are shown for the HC group (c) and AD group (d), for the peaks of the gain–minus-neutral difference wave (315 – 355 ms) and the loss-minus-neutral difference wave (555 – 575 ms) which occurred within the cue-P3 time window (300 – 600 ms). Note that ERPs are plotted with the negative y-axis pointing up. (e) Association between cue-P3a amplitudes (computed for the loss-minus-neutral difference wave over parietal electrodes [P1, P2, POz, Pz]) and OCDS scores. A single data point is included for each HC participant (N = 26) and AD participant (N = 20) who completed the online clinical questionnaires. The grey shaded region represents a 95% confidence interval for the linear regression lines shown in red (HC group) and blue (AD group).

For the cue-P3b we found a significant main effect of condition (F(2, 84) = 6.47, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.134) and a group by condition interaction (F(2, 84) = 4.18, p = 0.019, ηp2 = 0.091), but no main effect of group (F(1, 42) = 1.16, p = 0.289, ηp2 = 0.027). Tukey's post-hoc tests revealed cue-P3b amplitude was significantly larger for the gain condition compared to the neutral condition (p = 0.019) and loss condition compared to the neutral condition (p < 0.001) within the HC group but not the AD group (Fig. 2a and b). The HC group displayed a parietal distribution (Fig. 2c) aligned with prior eMIDT literature with controls [20,26], whereas the AD group exhibited a centro-frontal distribution (Fig. 2d). Negative sensor weightings (i.e., blue topography) within the AD group (Fig. 2d) indicate that neural processing for the neutral condition was elevated relative to the gain condition.

Association between OCDS score and loss-minus-neutral cue-P3a

The model testing for an association between OCDS score, group and loss-minus-neutral cue-P3a difference wave was significant (F(3, 42) = 15.05, pcorr < 0.001, R2 = 0.48). For the AD group, there was a significant negative association between OCDS score and cue-P3a (t = −3.43, pcorr = 0.002)(Fig. 2e). The group interaction term was significant (t = 2.96, pcorr = 0.005); and the follow-on regression confirmed cue-P3a was not associated with OCDS score in the HC group (Fig. 2e).

Effects of alcohol dependency on the CNV

We found a significant main effect of condition (F(2, 84) = 13.26, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.240). Tukey's post-hoc tests revealed CNV amplitude was significantly larger for the gain compared to the neutral condition (p < 0.001) and loss compared to the neutral condition (p < 0.001). We also found a main effect of group (F(1, 42) = 5.62, p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.118). Tukey's post-hoc tests revealed CNV amplitude was significantly larger in the AD group compared to the HC group, for the gain (p < 0.044), loss (p < 0.043), and neutral condition (p < 0.016) (Fig. 3a and b). The group by condition interaction was not significant (F(2, 84) = 0.48, p = 0.620, ηp2 = 0.011).

Figure 3

Fig. 3. ERP results for the CNV time window.Note: Average CNV ERP components for the HC group (a) and AD group (b) computed over central electrodes [C1, C2, FCz, Fz]. The grey shaded bars depict the time for the CNV. Scalp topographies are included for peak of the incentive–minus-neutral difference wave (1890 – 1920 ms) which occurred within the CNV time window (1800 – 2000 ms). ERPs are plotted with the negative y-axis pointing up. Regression plots are shown demonstrating the association between (c) gain amplitudes (computed over central electrodes [C1, C2, FCz, Fz]) and alcohol units consumed, and (d) CNV loss amplitudes (computed over central electrodes [C1, C2, FCz, Fz]) and alcohol units consumed. A single data point is included for each HC participant (N = 22) and AD participant (N = 21) who had consumed alcohol in their lives. The grey shaded region represents a 95% confidence interval for the linear regression lines shown in red (HC group) and blue (AD group).

Association between alcohol consumption and CNV

The models testing for an association between alcohol units consumed per adult year of drinking, group, CNV gain (F(3, 39) = 43.48, pcorr < 0.001, R2 = 0.75) and CNV loss (F(3, 39) = 15.05, pcorr < 0.001, R2 = 0.76) were significant. For the AD group, there was a significant negative association between CNV gain (t = −3.95, pcorr = 0.008)(Fig. 3c) and CNV loss (t = −3.79, pcorr < 0.001)(Fig. 3d). The group interaction terms were significant for both models (CNV gain: t = 3.45, pcorr = 0.002; CNV loss: t = 3.33, pcorr = 0.002), and follow-on regressions confirmed these ERP components were not associated with alcohol units in the HC group (Fig. 3c and d).

Effects of alcohol dependency on discrimination between loss and neutral cues

For the HC group, there was a sustained period of significant loss anticipation discrimination (loss-vs-neutral) spanning the entire cue-P3 window (i.e., 300 – 600 ms) (Fig. 4a), whereas the AD group did not exhibit significant loss discrimination following 500 ms (Fig. 4b). At the midpoint of the cue-P3b window (525 ms) the HC group had a parietal distribution which was absent in the AD group (Fig. 4a and b). Within the CNV window, the HC group demonstrated two peaks of significant discrimination (1830 ms, 1932 ms) which were absent in the AD group (Fig. 5a and b).

Figure 4

Fig. 4. LD-ML results for the cue-P3a and cue-P3b time windows.

Note: Results are averaged over all participants (mean line in blue ± se across participants, represented by the shaded blue area). The dotted red line represents the Az leading to a significance level of p = 0.05. The forward models (topographical scalp maps) are presented at the midpoint of the cue-P3a (375 ms) and cue-P3b (525 ms) time windows. (a) Single-trial linear discriminant machine learning (LD-ML) performance (Az) between loss and neutral cues as a function of cue-locked time for the HC group. (b) Single-trial LD-ML performance (Az) between loss and neutral cues as a function of cue-locked time for the AD group. (c) Single-trial LD-ML performance (Az) between gain and neutral cues as a function of cue-locked time for the HC group. (d) Single-trial LD-ML performance (Az) between gain and neutral cues as a function of cue-locked time for the AD group.

Figure 5

Fig. 5. LD-ML results for CNV time window.Note: Results are averaged over all participants (mean line in blue ± se across participants, represented by the shaded blue area). The dotted red line represents the Az leading to a significance level of p = 0.05. The forward models (topographical scalp maps) are presented at the midpoint of the CNV time window (1900 ms). (a) Single-trial linear discriminant machine learning (LD-ML) performance (Az) between loss and neutral cues as a function of cue-locked time for the HC group. (b) Single-trial LD-ML performance (Az) between loss and neutral cues as a function of cue-locked time for the AD group. (c) Single-trial LD_ML performance (Az) between gain and neutral cues as a function of cue-locked time for the HC group. (d) Single-trial LD-ML performance (Az) between gain and neutral cues as a function of cue-locked time for the AD group.

The SPM1d analyses revealed significant group differences in loss anticipation discrimination within the cue-P3a and cue-P3b window. Significant supra-threshold clusters were found between 328 – 350 ms (pcluster = 0.040, Z = 2.36), 354 – 367 ms (pcluster = 0.047, Z = 2.36) and 525 – 572 ms (pcluster = 0.022, Z = 2.32). Across suprathreshold time windows, Az values for loss anticipation discrimination were significantly larger in the HC group compared to the AD group. We found no significant between group differences in loss vs neutral discrimination within the CNV time window.

Effects of alcohol dependency on discrimination between gain and neutral cues

For both the HC and AD groups, we found a broad window of significant gain anticipation discrimination (gain-vs-neutral) spanning the entire window of cue-P3a and cue-P3b (Fig. 4c and d). At the midpoint of the cue-P3b time window (525 ms) the HC group demonstrated a parietal distribution (Fig. 4c). In contrast, the AD group had a centro-frontal distribution with negative sensor weightings (i.e., neural processing for neutral was elevated relative to gain) (Fig. 4d). Within the CNV window, the HC group and AD group exhibited a significant peak of discrimination at 1943 ms and 1830 ms, respectively (Fig. 5c and d). We found no significant between group differences in the magnitude of gain vs neutral discrimination across the cue-P3a, cue-P3b or CNV windows.

Discussion

Hypoactive cue-P3b response is evident in alcohol dependency

We aimed to investigate the temporal dynamics of reward dysfunction in alcohol dependence by using an EEG version of the monetary incentive delay task. As hypothesised, we found a hypoactive cue-P3 neural response to gain and loss anticipation vs neutral anticipation in AD compared to control participants. This occurred during the cue-P3b phase and therefore may reflect reduced memory processing of motivationally salient cues in alcohol dependency [18,23]. Whilst this finding of hypoactivation during anticipation of motivationally salient outcomes is consistent with prior fMRI-MIDT literature in substance dependence that also report blunted anticipatory reward processing [5], [6], [7], [8] it stands in contrast to other studies which do not report abnormal ventral striatal activity during reward anticipation in alcohol dependent individuals [9], [10], [11].

One possible reason for discrepant findings in neural responses during reward anticipation in AD may be MID task design. The large number (240 trials) of fast-paced low-incentive trials in our eMIDT (50 pence at stake for incentive trials, requiring a response every ̴5 s) may be more sensitive in revealing brain motivational deficits compared with the less demanding shorter fMRI-MIDT paradigms. The latter contained fewer than 100 slow paced trials (∼10 - 30 s) with medium ($1 - $5 stake) [9,10] or high incentives ($10 stake) [11]. Comparable task performance between HC and AD participants in the current study indicate AD participants were able to perform the eMIDT despite high attentional demands. Although the participants in [9] had a longer mean duration of abstinence than our study (36 months vs 25 months, respectively), similarly we did not find any associations between abstinence duration and neural signal of reward anticipation in exploratory regressions (see Supplementary Results). Taken together these findings suggest that abstinence duration to has little effect on the spatial and temporal aspects of the reward processing neural signal in alcohol dependency.

Another possible reason for contradictory findings between our study and prior research in long term abstinence [9] may be modality specific; we propose the high temporal resolution of EEG may be ideal for detecting subtle neural abnormalities that persist into protracted abstinence. In our AD group, it is possible that compensatory neural adaptations occurred to maintain long-term abstinence despite reward deficits. For example, the enhanced CNV response in our sample (discussed later) may have compensated for blunted cue-P3b response to ensure successful task performance. Whilst the millisecond resolution of EEG is ideal for detecting discrete neural events that occur in opposite directions (such as the cue-P3b and CNV), the poor temporal resolution of fMRI may conflate these neural events, preventing their detection.

Our LD-ML results were in agreement with our ERP findings for the cue-P3b. The AD group had significantly reduced LD-ML cue-P3b discriminator performance for loss anticipation (loss-vs-neutral) between 525 – 572 ms. Whilst we did not find group differences in the gain anticipation (gain-vs-neutral) discriminator performance which measures absolute differences in the EEG amplitudes, topographic plots revealed that effects were in opposite directions within each group. For the HC group, discrimination was driven by an enhanced neural response for gain compared to neutral cues, whereas in the AD group, discrimination was driven by enhancement for neutral compared to gain cues. The LD-ML and ERP analyses therefore corroborate one another and support the overall hypothesis that the appetitive cue-P3b response is blunted in AD. We propose this reduced P3b processing is a downstream consequence of dysfunctional ventral striatal processing as found in prior fMRI-MIDT studies in alcohol dependence. The eMIDT cue-P3b is thus highlighted as a neurophysiological marker of dependence, differentiating AD from HC even in protracted abstinence and could have application when investigating novel treatments for addiction.

Hypoactive cue-P3a loss anticipation is associated with increased OCDS score

The ERP analyses did not reveal group differences in anticipatory processing for the cue-P3a (300 – 450 ms) whilst the LD-ML analysis did reveal reduced loss vs neutral discrimination in this time period. Inconsistencies between the ERP and LD-ML results may be explained by enhanced SNR offered by the LD-ML approach [21,32] which linearly integrated data from all trials and electrodes across the whole brain rather than averaging data across trials (i.e., ERP). Although the cue-P3a ERP response did not significantly differ from controls when averaging data across the AD group, the regression analysis suggests variation within the group, such that AD participants with increased obsessive-compulsive thoughts about alcohol exhibit blunted cue-P3a loss anticipation. This finding is consistent with a prior fMRI-MIDT study in alcohol dependence that found a similar association between striatal activation and OCDS score [8]. The cue-P3a may therefore represent a neurophysiological marker of persistent vulnerability and relapse risk during protracted abstinence.

Hyperactive CNV is associated with increased alcohol consumption

During preparation for motor response, we observed a hyperactive CNV signal in alcohol dependence, with amplitudes elevated across all conditions, and greater hyperactivity associated with greater alcohol consumption. Since the AD participants in this study were not actively detoxifying from alcohol (i.e., they were all > 4 weeks abstinence duration) and exploratory regression analyses (see Supplementary Results) confirmed the CNV signal was not associated with abstinence duration, it is unlikely that this hyperactive CNV signal can be explained by CNS hyperexcitability which occurs during acute alcohol withdrawal [51].

Our results deviate from prior literature reporting reduced CNV amplitudes in dependent active drinkers [52] which may be explained by the differing clinical samples. Our cohort had maintained successful abstinence which relies upon neurobiological changes during the transition from dependence [53]. Despite EEG abnormalities uncovered in alcohol dependency, the groups had comparable RT and success rates. CNV hyperactivity discovered here may therefore indicate a compensatory mechanism, which manifests as more proactive motor inhibition and increased attentional allocation [54,55] in alcohol dependency to achieve the same behavioural eMIDT performance as controls. Potentially, participants who were most severely dependent (i.e., consumed more alcohol) exert greater attentional resources to respond accurately to the eMIDT target (which relies upon successful motor inhibition to prevent premature responses), resulting in the hyperactive CNV signal.

Prior fMRI motor inhibition studies with abstinent drug and alcohol dependent participants also report a hyperactive signal and unimpaired behavioural responses, hypothesised to reflect recovery of cognitive control mechanisms underlying successful abstinence [37,56]. Further longitudinal research is required to determine whether CNV hyperactivity is a risk marker for high alcohol consumption, or conversely, represents enhanced control mechanisms required to maintain abstinence despite severe dependence.

Strengths and limitations

Recruitment of an abstinent medication free AD group allowed us to isolate reward dysfunction associated with alcohol dependency and ensure exclusion of the effects of psychoactive substances. This study faced recruitment challenges due to stringent exclusion criteria and covid-19 pandemic lockdowns, which led to a relatively low sample size. There was a wide range in abstinence duration (from 1 month to 130 months) within the AD group which consisted of 21 participants. Notably, the mean abstinence duration was 25 months and most participants (14 out of 21) had been abstinent for 23 months or less. Exploratory analyses (see Supplementary Results) confirmed there were no associations between cue-P3a, cue-P3b or CNV data and duration of abstinence, thus this study cannot make inferences about the impact of protracted vs. short-term abstinence on reward-related EEG measures. The long duration of abstinence within some participants may call into question the reliability of the alcohol consumption measures reported. However, although AD participants had not been actively drinking for months or years, in general they had experience of reporting their alcohol consumption in the time leading up to their abstinence, which is an important and memorable part of the alcohol recovery process, and thus they could easily recall these measures during the clinical assessment process.

Despite these limitations we demonstrated EEG to be a sensitive low-cost approach for detecting reward processing dysfunction in alcohol dependence with both ERP and LD-ML methods. We recommend the clinical utility of the proposed eMIDT risk markers to be validated in large, clinically representative samples (e.g., predict relapse, treatment, progression to alcohol use disorder in ‘at risk’ drinkers). Machine learning analysis of a dataset of hundreds of young adults, including fMRI-MIDT functional markers, personality, cognitive and environmental factors, have been successful at predicting future adolescent alcohol misuse [57]. However, translation into the clinical domain have been limited. It has been argued that much larger training datasets are needed for clinical translatability [58]. Due to the widespread availability, portability, and cost-effectiveness and sensitivity of EEG, we propose the eMIDT to be suitable for future big data approaches to clinical diagnosis and risk prediction.

Conclusion

Here, we demonstrated eMIDT sensitivity to a hypoactive cue-P3 response to gain and loss anticipation in alcohol dependency, supporting the ‘reward deficiency’ theory of addiction. In contrast, during preparation for motor response we found increased alcohol consumption was associated with a general hyperactive CNV signal in alcohol dependency. The eMIDT markers of alcohol dependency identified here hold potential for big data approaches to clinical diagnosis, prognosis, and clinical translation.

Abstract

A wealth of functional magnetic resonance imaging monetary incentive delay task (MIDT) research has shown alcohol dependency is associated with a hypoactive striatal response during gain anticipation (gain > neutral) and loss anticipation (loss > neutral). Electroencephalography (EEG) holds clinical advantages over fMRI (high temporal resolution, low cost, portable) however its use to study reward processing in alcohol dependence is limited. We aimed to carry out the first EEG MIDT (eMIDT) study in alcohol dependence. 21 abstinent alcohol dependent individuals and 26 controls performed an MIDT while neural activity was recorded using 64-channel EEG. Trial averaged event-related potentials (ERPs) and single-trial machine learning discriminant analyses were applied to EEG data. Clinical variables related to severity of dependence were collected and relationships with ERP data explored. Alcohol dependent individuals, compared with healthy controls, had blunted cue-P3 amplitudes for gain and loss anticipation (interaction: p = 0.019); and elevated contingent negative variation amplitudes for all conditions (gain, loss, neutral)(main effect: p < 0.001) which was associated with increased alcohol consumption (p = 0.002). The machine learning analyses demonstrated alcohol dependent individuals had reduced ability to discriminate between loss and neutral cues between 328 – 350 ms (p = 0.040), 354 – 367 ms (p = 0.047) and 525 – 572 ms (p = 0.022). The eMIDT approach is demonstrated to be a low-cost, sensitive measure of dysfunctional anticipatory reward processing in alcohol dependence, which we propose is ideal for big data approaches to prognostic psychiatry and translation into clinical practice.

Summary

Convergent research indicates disrupted neurobiological reward networks contribute to addiction vulnerability and maintenance. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) monetary incentive delay task (MIDT) studies, while informative, exhibit inconsistent findings regarding reward processing in substance dependence due to limitations in temporal resolution. Electroencephalography (EEG), offering superior temporal resolution, provides a cost-effective alternative for investigating reward-related neural processes. This study, the first to utilize an EEG version of the MIDT (eMIDT) in alcohol dependence, aimed to investigate anticipatory reward processing with high temporal precision using both traditional event-related potential (ERP) analysis and a machine learning approach.

Participants

The study comprised 21 abstinent alcohol-dependent (AD) participants and 26 healthy controls (HC), matched for age and sex. Participants met established inclusion/exclusion criteria, including DSM-V criteria for alcohol use disorder and abstinence duration exceeding four weeks. Ethical approval was obtained prior to study commencement.

Experimental Sessions and Design

Participants underwent clinical screening, including substance use assessments, before participating in an eMIDT task. This involved responding to visual cues predicting monetary gains, losses, or neutral outcomes. A staircase algorithm adjusted target durations to maintain a target accuracy of approximately 66%. EEG data were acquired during task performance.

EEG Data Acquisition and Pre-processing

EEG data (64 channels) were recorded at 1000 Hz, subsequently down-sampled and filtered. Channel and artifact rejection was performed using established methods. Independent component analysis (ICA) reduced dimensionality, and ERPlab extracted epochs for subsequent analysis. Epoch rejection was avoided to maximize data availability for analysis and to retain trial-by-trial variability.

Event-Related Potential (ERP) Analyses

ERPs, including cue-P3a, cue-P3b, and contingent negative variation (CNV), were analyzed using mixed-model ANCOVAs, controlling for age, IQ, and smoking status. Post-hoc tests investigated condition-specific effects. Linear regression models explored relationships between ERP amplitudes and clinical variables.

Machine Learning Analyses

Multivariate linear discriminant machine learning (LD-ML) assessed single-trial EEG data across gain and loss anticipation conditions. Area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (Az) values were calculated using leave-one-trial-out cross-validation. Group differences in Az values were compared using SPM1d.

Results

Behavioral results showed no significant group differences in reaction times, despite the high attentional demands of the eMIDT. ERP analysis revealed a hypoactive cue-P3b response in the AD group during both gain and loss anticipation. This hypoactivity correlated with obsessive-compulsive thoughts about alcohol. A hyperactive CNV was observed in the AD group, correlating with lifetime alcohol consumption. LD-ML analysis supported the ERP findings, demonstrating reduced loss anticipation discrimination in the AD group during the cue-P3b window.

Discussion

The hypoactive cue-P3b in alcohol dependence supports the reward deficiency hypothesis of addiction. Discrepancies with prior fMRI studies may reflect differences in task design and temporal resolution. The hyperactive CNV might represent a compensatory mechanism for maintaining task performance. The study's limitations include sample size and the range of abstinence durations. Nonetheless, the findings demonstrate EEG's utility for detecting subtle reward processing abnormalities in alcohol dependence, suggesting potential for clinical applications.

Conclusion

This study highlights the eMIDT's sensitivity in detecting reward processing dysfunction in alcohol dependence. The identified EEG markers hold promise for future large-scale studies aiming to improve clinical diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment development.

Abstract

A wealth of functional magnetic resonance imaging monetary incentive delay task (MIDT) research has shown alcohol dependency is associated with a hypoactive striatal response during gain anticipation (gain > neutral) and loss anticipation (loss > neutral). Electroencephalography (EEG) holds clinical advantages over fMRI (high temporal resolution, low cost, portable) however its use to study reward processing in alcohol dependence is limited. We aimed to carry out the first EEG MIDT (eMIDT) study in alcohol dependence. 21 abstinent alcohol dependent individuals and 26 controls performed an MIDT while neural activity was recorded using 64-channel EEG. Trial averaged event-related potentials (ERPs) and single-trial machine learning discriminant analyses were applied to EEG data. Clinical variables related to severity of dependence were collected and relationships with ERP data explored. Alcohol dependent individuals, compared with healthy controls, had blunted cue-P3 amplitudes for gain and loss anticipation (interaction: p = 0.019); and elevated contingent negative variation amplitudes for all conditions (gain, loss, neutral)(main effect: p < 0.001) which was associated with increased alcohol consumption (p = 0.002). The machine learning analyses demonstrated alcohol dependent individuals had reduced ability to discriminate between loss and neutral cues between 328 – 350 ms (p = 0.040), 354 – 367 ms (p = 0.047) and 525 – 572 ms (p = 0.022). The eMIDT approach is demonstrated to be a low-cost, sensitive measure of dysfunctional anticipatory reward processing in alcohol dependence, which we propose is ideal for big data approaches to prognostic psychiatry and translation into clinical practice.

Summary

Converging research indicates that disruptions in brain reward networks increase addiction vulnerability. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies using the monetary incentive delay task (MIDT) have yielded inconsistent results regarding reward processing in substance dependence. Electroencephalography (EEG), offering superior temporal resolution, presents an alternative approach. This study employed an EEG version of the MIDT (eMIDT) to investigate reward processing in alcohol-dependent (AD) individuals compared to healthy controls (HC). The researchers hypothesized that AD participants would show hypoactive cue-P3 and hyperactive contingent negative variation (CNV) responses, correlating with clinical symptoms.

Participants

The study included 21 abstinent AD participants (13 male) and 26 HC participants (17 male). AD participants met DSM-V criteria for severe alcohol use disorder and were abstinent for at least four weeks. HC participants had no history of substance dependence (except nicotine). All participants underwent a clinical screening, including EEG recording. The study received ethics approval.

Experimental Sessions and Design

Participants completed an eMIDT involving cue stimuli (gain, loss, neutral) followed by a target requiring a motor response. A staircase algorithm adjusted target duration to maintain accuracy around 66%. Feedback followed, reflecting monetary gains or losses. The task involved 240 trials across five blocks.

Experimental Stimuli and EEG Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

The eMIDT stimuli were validated images. The EEG was recorded using 64 active electrodes. Preprocessing involved downsampling, filtering, channel rejection/interpolation, noise removal using various plug-ins, Independent Component Analysis (ICA), and epoch extraction. Epoch rejection was avoided to maximize data for analysis.

Event-Related Potential (ERP) Analyses

Cue-P3 was analyzed as cue-P3a (300–450 ms) and cue-P3b (450–600 ms) subcomponents over parietal electrodes, and CNV (1800–2000 ms) over centro-frontal electrodes. Mixed-model analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were performed, controlling for age, IQ, and smoking status. Associations between ERP data and clinical variables (lifetime alcohol consumption and Obsessive Compulsive Drinking Scale (OCDS) scores) were explored using linear regression.

Machine Learning Analyses

Multivariate linear discriminant machine learning (LD-ML) was used to decode experimental conditions across the whole brain. Area under the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve (Az) was calculated using leave-one-trial-out cross-validation. Group differences in Az values were compared using SPM1d with random field theory (RFT) for threshold determination.

Results: Behavioral Responses

Reaction times (RTs) were significantly shorter for gain and loss conditions compared to neutral conditions. No significant group differences or interactions were found for RT.

Results: Effects of Alcohol Dependency on ERP Components

A hypoactive cue-P3b response to gain and loss anticipation was observed in the AD group compared to the HC group. The AD group showed a centro-frontal, rather than parietal, distribution of cue-P3b. A negative association was found between cue-P3a (loss-minus-neutral) and OCDS scores in the AD group, but not in the HC group. The AD group showed a hyperactive CNV response across all conditions, correlating with higher lifetime alcohol consumption.

Results: Machine Learning Analyses

LD-ML confirmed reduced loss anticipation discrimination in the cue-P3b window for the AD group. Topographic maps revealed opposite patterns in gain anticipation between the groups. No significant group differences were found in gain versus neutral discrimination.

Discussion

The findings support the reward deficiency hypothesis of addiction. The high temporal resolution of EEG may be crucial for detecting subtle neural abnormalities persisting into protracted abstinence. The hyperactive CNV might represent a compensatory mechanism maintaining task performance. The study's limitations include sample size and the range in abstinence duration. Despite these limitations, EEG's cost-effectiveness and sensitivity make it promising for large-scale studies of addiction.

Conclusion

This study demonstrates the eMIDT's sensitivity to reward processing dysfunction in alcohol dependence. The identified EEG markers hold potential for improved clinical diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment development.

Abstract

A wealth of functional magnetic resonance imaging monetary incentive delay task (MIDT) research has shown alcohol dependency is associated with a hypoactive striatal response during gain anticipation (gain > neutral) and loss anticipation (loss > neutral). Electroencephalography (EEG) holds clinical advantages over fMRI (high temporal resolution, low cost, portable) however its use to study reward processing in alcohol dependence is limited. We aimed to carry out the first EEG MIDT (eMIDT) study in alcohol dependence. 21 abstinent alcohol dependent individuals and 26 controls performed an MIDT while neural activity was recorded using 64-channel EEG. Trial averaged event-related potentials (ERPs) and single-trial machine learning discriminant analyses were applied to EEG data. Clinical variables related to severity of dependence were collected and relationships with ERP data explored. Alcohol dependent individuals, compared with healthy controls, had blunted cue-P3 amplitudes for gain and loss anticipation (interaction: p = 0.019); and elevated contingent negative variation amplitudes for all conditions (gain, loss, neutral)(main effect: p < 0.001) which was associated with increased alcohol consumption (p = 0.002). The machine learning analyses demonstrated alcohol dependent individuals had reduced ability to discriminate between loss and neutral cues between 328 – 350 ms (p = 0.040), 354 – 367 ms (p = 0.047) and 525 – 572 ms (p = 0.022). The eMIDT approach is demonstrated to be a low-cost, sensitive measure of dysfunctional anticipatory reward processing in alcohol dependence, which we propose is ideal for big data approaches to prognostic psychiatry and translation into clinical practice.

Summary

Research suggests that disruptions in brain reward centers increase the risk and sustain addiction. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, while useful, have limitations in their temporal resolution. Electroencephalography (EEG), offering superior temporal resolution, presents an alternative for studying reward processing. This study uses EEG to examine reward processing in individuals with alcohol dependence (AD) compared to healthy controls (HC).

Participants

The study included 21 abstinent AD participants (13 male) and 26 HC participants (17 male). All participants had normal or corrected vision and hearing and were English speakers. AD participants met criteria for severe alcohol use disorder and were abstinent for at least four weeks. HC participants had no history of alcohol or drug dependence (except nicotine). The study was approved by the University of Huddersfield's ethics committee.

Experimental Sessions

Participants underwent telephone and in-person screenings. Screenings included written consent, breathalyzer and urine tests, interviews, and questionnaires. Eligible participants completed an EEG recording session, including practice and full eMIDT trials. Participants were compensated for their time.

Experimental Design

Participants completed an EEG version of the monetary incentive delay (MID) task. The task involved cues indicating potential monetary gain, loss, or neutrality, followed by a response task and feedback. A staircase algorithm adjusted the difficulty of the response task to maintain accuracy around 66%.

EEG Data Acquisition and Pre-processing

EEG data were recorded from 64 electrodes. Pre-processing involved downsampling, filtering, channel rejection (with interpolation), noise reduction, independent component analysis (ICA), and epoch extraction. Epoch rejection was avoided to maximize data and statistical power.

ERP Analyses

Cue-P3a (300–450 ms) and cue-P3b (450–600 ms) components were analyzed. Analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) assessed group differences, controlling for age, IQ, and smoking status.

Associations Between ERP Data and Clinical Severity

Linear regression models explored associations between ERP data (difference waves and mean amplitudes) and clinical variables (lifetime alcohol consumption and Obsessive Compulsive Drinking Scale scores).

Machine Learning Analyses

Multivariate linear discriminant machine learning (LD-ML) was used to analyze single-trial data. This approach aimed to improve signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) by decoding experimental conditions across the whole brain.

Behavioural Responses

Reaction times (RTs) were significantly faster for gain and loss trials than for neutral trials. No significant group differences or interactions were found for RT.

Effects of Alcohol Dependency on the Cue-P3

The cue-P3a showed a main effect of condition, but no group effect or interaction. The cue-P3b showed a main effect of condition and a group-by-condition interaction; the AD group showed less differentiation between conditions than the HC group.

Association Between OCDS Score and Loss-Minus-Neutral Cue-P3a

A significant negative association was found between OCDS scores and cue-P3a amplitudes in the AD group, but not in the HC group.

Effects of Alcohol Dependency on the CNV

The CNV showed a main effect of condition and a main effect of group; the AD group exhibited larger CNV amplitudes across conditions than the HC group.

Association Between Alcohol Consumption and CNV

Significant negative associations were found between alcohol consumption and CNV amplitudes (gain and loss) in the AD group, but not in the HC group.

Effects of Alcohol Dependency on Discrimination Between Loss and Neutral Cues

The HC group showed sustained loss anticipation discrimination across the cue-P3 window; the AD group showed reduced discrimination after 500 ms. No significant group differences were found for the CNV window.

Effects of Alcohol Dependency on Discrimination Between Gain and Neutral Cues

Both groups showed gain anticipation discrimination across the cue-P3 window, but with different topographical distributions. No significant group differences were found for the CNV window.

Discussion

The study found evidence of a hypoactive cue-P3b response to gain and loss anticipation in AD, consistent with the reward deficiency theory of addiction. The hyperactive CNV response may represent a compensatory mechanism. Limitations include sample size and the range of abstinence duration.

Strengths and Limitations

The study's strengths include the use of an abstinent, medication-free AD group and the use of both ERP and LD-ML analyses. Limitations include the relatively small sample size and the wide range of abstinence durations.

Conclusion

This study demonstrates the sensitivity of eMIDT in detecting reward processing dysfunction in alcohol dependence. The findings suggest that eMIDT markers could be valuable for clinical diagnosis and prediction.

Abstract

A wealth of functional magnetic resonance imaging monetary incentive delay task (MIDT) research has shown alcohol dependency is associated with a hypoactive striatal response during gain anticipation (gain > neutral) and loss anticipation (loss > neutral). Electroencephalography (EEG) holds clinical advantages over fMRI (high temporal resolution, low cost, portable) however its use to study reward processing in alcohol dependence is limited. We aimed to carry out the first EEG MIDT (eMIDT) study in alcohol dependence. 21 abstinent alcohol dependent individuals and 26 controls performed an MIDT while neural activity was recorded using 64-channel EEG. Trial averaged event-related potentials (ERPs) and single-trial machine learning discriminant analyses were applied to EEG data. Clinical variables related to severity of dependence were collected and relationships with ERP data explored. Alcohol dependent individuals, compared with healthy controls, had blunted cue-P3 amplitudes for gain and loss anticipation (interaction: p = 0.019); and elevated contingent negative variation amplitudes for all conditions (gain, loss, neutral)(main effect: p < 0.001) which was associated with increased alcohol consumption (p = 0.002). The machine learning analyses demonstrated alcohol dependent individuals had reduced ability to discriminate between loss and neutral cues between 328 – 350 ms (p = 0.040), 354 – 367 ms (p = 0.047) and 525 – 572 ms (p = 0.022). The eMIDT approach is demonstrated to be a low-cost, sensitive measure of dysfunctional anticipatory reward processing in alcohol dependence, which we propose is ideal for big data approaches to prognostic psychiatry and translation into clinical practice.

Summary

Scientists studied how the brains of people with alcohol problems respond to rewards. They used a special test called eMIDT that measures brain waves. They compared people who had alcohol problems (the AD group) to people without alcohol problems (the HC group).

Brain Waves and Rewards

The scientists looked at two types of brain waves: cue-P3 and CNV. Cue-P3 shows how the brain reacts to the chance of winning or losing something. CNV shows how the brain gets ready to act.

Alcohol Problems and Brain Waves

The study found that people with alcohol problems had weaker cue-P3 brain waves when thinking about winning or losing. This means their brains weren't reacting as strongly to rewards. They also had stronger CNV brain waves, possibly showing that their brains were working harder to focus. The more alcohol someone had drunk in the past, the stronger their CNV waves were.

The eMIDT Test

The scientists used the eMIDT test, which is like a video game where people try to win or avoid losing money. It's a quicker and cheaper test than others, making it easier to use for more people.

What the Study Means

The study shows that eMIDT can be a helpful tool to see how alcohol problems affect the brain. The findings help scientists understand how alcohol problems change how the brain reacts to rewards and might help develop better ways to treat these problems.

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Komarnyckyj, M., Retzler, C., Whelan, R., Young, O., Fouragnan, E., & Murphy, A. (2023). Anticipatory reward dysfunction in alcohol dependence: An electroencephalography monetary incentive delay task study. Addiction Neuroscience, 8, 100116.

    Highlights