Adolescence and Reward: Making Sense of Neural and Behavioral Changes Amid the Chaos
Deena M. Walker
Margaret R. Bell
Cecilia Flores
Joshua M. Gulley
Jari Willing
SummaryOriginal

Summary

Teenagers' heightened susceptibility to rewards and risk-taking (e.g., drugs) stems from brain changes, hormones, social interactions, and environment, potentially shaping future brain development.

2017

Adolescence and Reward: Making Sense of Neural and Behavioral Changes Amid the Chaos

Keywords Adolescence; Drugs; Emotions; Thinking skills; Risks; Rewards; Impulses; Brain development; Mental health; Drugs of abuse

Abstract

During the teen years and young adulthood, people develop more social, emotional, and thinking skills, but also take more risks, like trying drugs. This happens in part because young people are more sensitive to rewards and have less self-control. Difficulty during this time of life can lead to long-lasting changes in how the brain continues to change into adulthood. This review discusses how connections in the brain, hormones during puberty, social situations, and the environment affect how young people learn to respond to rewards.

Introduction

Adolescence can be both an exciting and tumultuous time. It comprises the formative years during which individuals reach sexual maturity and develop social, emotional, and cognitive skills needed as individuals move toward independence and adulthood (Spear, 2000). It is a time of increased exploration, but this exploration often includes increased sensation seeking and the initiation of drug use (Steinberg, 2004; Lipari and Jean-Francois, 2013), which could contribute to the high percentage of preventable deaths among teens (Miniño, 2010). It is also a time of increased vulnerability to stress and the emergence of several psychiatric and behavioral disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, depression, eating disorders) (Kessler et al., 2005). Hence, research into the neurobiological underpinnings of adolescence is important for basic understanding of normative social, emotional, reproductive, and cognitive development as well as for understanding, prevention, and treatment of health risks and disorders that characterize this life stage.

The reorganization of the reward circuitry during adolescence is one factor that is integral to adolescent development and increased vulnerability to disease (Luciana, 2013; Doremus-Fitzwater and Spear, 2016). This process is driven by complex interactions between neural pathways, endocrine axes, and environmental stimuli to produce a functional mesocorticolimbic reward system in adulthood. Hence, it is imperative to determine how these factors act independently and in concert to shape the mesocorticolimbic reward circuitry during adolescence. This review highlights research on interactions between the mesocorticolimbic dopamine (DA) system, pubertal hormones, and environmental perturbations (drug use and social stress) and their effects on cognitive and social adolescent development.

Puberty-dependent and puberty-independent adolescent development

Puberty and adolescence both refer to the transition from childhood to adulthood, but these terms are not equivalent. Puberty is reserved for physiological and behavioral changes associated with the attainment of reproductive competence (e.g., activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal [HPG] axis, appearance of secondary sex characteristics, and onset of sexual interest and mating behaviors). Adolescence is a broader term that includes puberty as well as nonreproductive traits (e.g., social, emotional, and cognitive development). Reproductive hormones, however, can have widespread effects, and the development of several nonreproductive adolescent traits can also be driven by activation of the HPG axis at puberty (puberty-dependent; e.g., ethanol intake, anxiety-related behaviors) (Primus and Kellogg, 1989, 1990; Vetter-O'Hagen and Spear, 2011). The different physiological, anatomical, and temporal changes of puberty between males and females can also lead to the emergence of sex differences during adolescence (Schulz et al., 2009). Other adolescent traits, however, develop independently of HPG activation and merely coincide with pubertal development (puberty-independent; e.g., social play, aggression) (Whitsett, 1975; Smith et al., 1996; Wommack and Delville, 2007). Sex differences may also manifest in puberty-independent traits due to organizational actions of perinatal hormones or direct actions of genes on the sex chromosomes (Arnold, 2017). This puberty-dependent versus puberty-independent distinction is important because many neuropsychiatric and behavioral disorders arise during adolescence, exhibit striking sex differences, and are impacted by pubertal hormones as well as nonpubertal factors (Fombonne, 2009; Graber, 2013; Trotman et al., 2013). Given that the mesocorticolimbic DA pathway is sexually dimorphic (Becker, 2009) and regulated by gonadal hormones in adults (Kuhn et al., 2010; Becker et al., 2012), a central question is whether adolescent development of reward-related behaviors and circuitry is puberty-dependent or puberty-independent.

Sex differences in reward and reward-related circuitry

Studies in humans and laboratory animals generally support the notion that adolescents are more sensitive to reward than adults. This is behaviorally manifest in multiple ways, including elevated levels of sensation seeking and risk taking, as well as reduced inhibitory control, which are maximal during the early to mid-adolescent period (Burnett et al., 2010; Andrzejewski et al., 2011; Burton and Fletcher, 2012; Urošević et al., 2012; Collado et al., 2014). In laboratory rodents, heightened reactivity to drug rewards has also been demonstrated (Doremus et al., 2005; Levin et al., 2007; Anker and Carroll, 2010), although this might depend on the drug or other procedural factors (Doremus-Fitzwater and Spear, 2016). When gender or sex is considered, an even more nuanced picture emerges. For example, compared with males, females have a relatively earlier and lower magnitude peak in sensation seeking during mid-adolescence that is followed by a more rapid decline to stability by early adulthood (Shulman et al., 2015). In this comprehensive, longitudinal study, it was also demonstrated that impulse control improved steadily following early adolescence, but males remained more impulsive than females through their mid-20s. In rats, male adolescents exhibit greater intake and motivation for palatable food that is either calorie dense (sweetened condensed milk) (Friemel et al., 2010) or calorie devoid (Marshall et al., 2017), compared with adults. However, this age-dependent difference in reward sensitivity was not apparent in female rats (Marshall et al., 2017). Using food-restricted rats trained to associate a tone with delivery of a sucrose solution, Hammerslag and Gulley (2014) found that the effects of age and sex were dependent on the characteristics of the behavior being measured. Specifically, females exhibited enhanced development of stimulus-directed behavior in that both adult and adolescent females acquired Pavlovian approach more quickly than males. Adolescents of both sexes, however, had weaker expression of goal-directed behavior (i.e., entries into the sucrose delivery trough) and were less sensitive to reward devaluation than adults. Recent work has also highlighted gender and sex differences in neural development of reward-related brain circuits that may play an important role in these age and gender/sex differences in behavior. In the striatum, adolescent boys lag behind as they reach peak striatal volume at ∼15 years of age compared with 12 for girls (Raznahan et al., 2014). Structural development in the cortex also appears relatively delayed in boys compared with girls, although exceptions include a more rapid reduction in the thickness of the dorsolateral PFC in males (Raznahan et al., 2010). Many of these adolescent cortical changes are associated with adrenal and/or gonadal markers of pubertal maturation, often in a sex-dependent manner (e.g., Herting et al., 2014, 2017). In the rat mPFC, there are significant decreases in neuron number (Markham et al., 2007), dendritic complexity (Koss et al., 2014), and synapse number (Drzewiecki et al., 2016) between adolescence and adulthood. At least some of these changes are more pronounced in females compared with males and are closely linked to puberty onset (Willing and Juraska, 2015). In the core and shell regions of the NAc, these “pruning” processes and the emergence of adult-like morphological features appear to occur much earlier and well before the onset of puberty (Tepper et al., 1998; Lee and Sawatari, 2011).

Development of the PFC during adolescence

The mPFC is a crucial regulator of reward-directed behaviors and likely contributes to cognitive development during adolescence. As a major component of the mesocorticolimbic DA pathway, it receives dopaminergic projections from the VTA and sends key glutamatergic projections to the NAc, a key integrator of reward processing (Albertin et al., 2000; McGinty and Grace, 2009; Hamel et al., 2017; Morrison et al., 2017). These regions form a larger circuitry that includes the BLA and ventral hippocampus, among others (Fig. 1). This circuit acts in concert to modulate dopaminergic and glutamatergic tone integrated by the NAc in response to salient stimuli. Loss or reduction of signaling within the PFC in humans has been associated with numerous psychiatric disorders, including anxiety and depression (Ressler and Mayberg, 2007) and substance use disorders (Volkow et al., 2010) in adulthood. Similar effects have been observed in rodent models where exposure to stress or drugs of abuse can influence signaling between the PFC and NAc, resulting in addiction-related behaviors (MacAskill et al., 2014) or depressive-related behaviors (Covington et al., 2010; Vialou et al., 2014; Bagot et al., 2015). For example, repeated exposure to cocaine in adult mice decreases the PFC inputs to D1 DA receptor containing medium spiny neurons in the NAc (MacAskill et al., 2014).

Figure 1. Adolescent development of the mesocorticolimbic DA pathway. A, Major brain areas and projections of the mesocorticolimbic DA pathway. B, A schematic of postnatal development of key components of this pathway along with changes in gonadal steroid hormones and pubertal markers. Hashed lines indicate data specific to females. Developmental patterns and markers are based on data from Tarazi and Baldessarini (2000) (NAc D1, D2 receptors); Naneix et al. (2012) and Willing et al. (2017) (PFC DA fibers); and Döhler and Wuttke (1975) and Vetter-O'Hagen and Spear (2012a) (gonadal steroid concentrations and pubertal markers). VO, Vaginal opening; BPS, balano-preputial separation; E2, estradiol; T, testosterone. One of the most dramatic brain changes occurring during adolescence is the unfolding of DA connectivity in the mPFC. In contrast to DA projections to limbic regions (e.g., NAc) and cortical innervation of other monoamines (e.g., norepinephrine and serotonin) that reach adult density levels early in life (Coyle and Molliver, 1977; Levitt and Moore, 1979; Lidov et al., 1980; Benes et al., 2000; Diamond, 2002), DA projections to the mPFC do not fully mature until early adulthood (Kalsbeek et al., 1988; Benes et al., 2000; Manitt et al., 2011; Naneix et al., 2012). In rodent models, the number of dopaminergic fibers in the mPFC increases linearly between the juvenile period (postnatal day [P] 25) and young adulthood, with the most prominent increases occurring between the late juvenile period and early adulthood (Naneix et al., 2012; Willing et al., 2017). Interestingly, this is not a rodent-specific phenomenon as protracted mesocortical DA development occurs in nonhuman primates and most likely in humans (Rosenberg and Lewis, 1994; Lambe et al., 2000), paralleling cognitive maturation. In addition to changes in dopaminergic projections in adolescence, changes in dopaminergic receptor expression are prevalent throughout the mesocorticolimbic system, which may underlie the altered sensitivity to rewarding stimuli. In the NAc and dorsal striatum, DA D1 and D2 receptor expression peaks during adolescence (P40), then declines to reach adult levels ∼P80 (Andersen et al., 2000). In the PFC of rats, there is also a selective decrease of cells expressing D1 receptors that project to the NAc between P44 and adulthood (Brenhouse et al., 2008). However, there may be important species differences in these receptor changes across adolescence (Pokinko et al., 2017). Measures of functional connectivity in humans have further elucidated the widespread changes between the PFC and subcortical structures throughout adolescence, with some evidence suggesting that a relatively stable network connectivity state does not occur until at least the mid-20s (Dosenbach et al., 2010). The potential relevance of these changes for behavior is not fully understood, but decreases in the functional coupling between subregions of the PFC and the NAc have been linked with decreases in self-reported risky behavior across adolescence (Qu et al., 2015). Interestingly, studies of PFC activation in humans have revealed sex differences in function that go beyond what might be expected from the anatomical correlates. One such functional development is resting state functional connectivity, which describes the degree of synchrony between two different brain regions or between nearby areas within a brain region. In the dorsolateral PFC, resting state functional connectivity between the hemispheres tends to increase with age in males but decreases with age in females (Zuo et al., 2010). These data demonstrate that the mesocorticolimbic DA pathway undergoes vast developmental changes during adolescence both in fiber projections to the PFC and in sensitivity to DA within the NAc, dorsal striatum, and PFC target areas through altered receptor expression. Some of these developmental changes seem to occur independent of the gonadal hormone surge associated with puberty (Andersen et al., 2000; Willing et al., 2017). Although tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactivity in the PFC increases across adolescence, this increase does not appear to be associated with markers of pubertal status (Willing et al., 2017). Preventing the pubertal rise in gonadal hormones by gonadectomy on P28 does not alter the adolescent (P40) or adult (P80) levels of D1 or D2 receptor expression in the rat striatum (Andersen et al., 2000). Finally, many developmental changes occur before puberty (e.g., adult-like morphological features of striatal neurons) (Tepper et al., 1998; Lee and Sawatari, 2011). For many measures, more research is needed to answer this question. The influence of gonadal hormones on reward-associated behaviors and the mesocorticolimbic pathway in adults (Becker et al., 2012) suggests at least a modulatory role during adolescence, particularly with respect to the emergence of sex differences (Kuhn et al., 2010).

Pubertal influences on mPFC adolescent development

Recent evidence suggests that, within the adolescent period, pubertal onset may be particularly critical in specific aspects of mPFC development and cognition. Previous work in rats has documented a reduction in mPFC volume between the juvenile and adult periods (Van Eden and Uylings, 1985), and this volumetric reduction may reflect a decrease in neuron number. Stereological quantification of the total number of neurons in the mPFC across adolescence revealed that the majority of neuronal losses occur during the period of pubertal onset, particularly in female rats (Willing and Juraska, 2015). Ovariectomy before puberty prevented these neuronal losses, further suggesting a role for pubertal hormones (Koss et al., 2015). Additionally, there are changes in dendritic complexity and synapse number in the mPFC during adolescence. Between P35 and P90, there is a reduction in dendritic spine density in both male and female rats (Koss et al., 2014). In a recent study, Drzewiecki et al. (2016) conducted an immunohistochemical analysis of synaptophysin as a marker for total synapse number in the mPFC in P25, P35, P45, P60, and P90 rats of both sexes. As expected, there was evidence for significant synaptic pruning during adolescence. Interestingly, a direct comparison of prepubertal versus postpubertal females at P35 and prepubertal versus postpubertal males at P45 (corresponding to the average age of pubertal onset) revealed that, in both sexes, postpubertal animals had significantly fewer synapses than their prepubertal counterparts.These structural alterations within the mPFC are associated with changes in cognitive performance during adolescence, which also seem to depend on the timing of puberty. These differences in cognitive performance could reflect differences in reward processing. Indeed, substance use disorder is often described as maladaptive decision-making and reward learning. Given the importance of the entire PFC in reward learning, it follows that structural changes in adolescence result in altered cognitive performance and decision making with regard to reward. Kanit et al. (2000) found that pubertal onset alters learning strategies in spatial memory tasks. However, there is a paucity of research that accounts for a potential role for puberty, particularly on mPFC-dependent tasks. Willing et al. (2016) have recently shown that pubertal onset leads to better performance on an mPFC-mediated cognitive flexibility component of a Morris Water Maze task in both male and female rats. Path length to the novel platform location was shorter in postpubertal males and females, and prepubertal animals spent a greater amount of time swimming in the quadrant where the platform was initially located, suggesting a deficit in cognitive flexibility that subsides after pubertal onset. Future studies are needed to determine whether these temporal associations with pubertal status reflect pubertal mechanisms or coincidental timing. Recent studies in female mice support the former. Preventing the pubertal rise in ovarian hormones by prepubertal gonadectomy blocks adolescent maturation of inhibitory neurotransmission in the frontal cortex (Piekarski et al., 2017). Conversely, advancing puberty by prepubertal estradiol and progesterone treatment accelerates the maturation of this inhibitory tone in the frontal cortex and developmental increases in cognitive flexibility (Piekarski et al., 2017).

Disruption of adolescent PFC development by drugs of abuse

The initiation of consumption of drugs of abuse is an adolescent phenomenon, with >50% of individuals taking drugs for the first time being below the age of 18 years (Johnston and Coplin, 1979; Warner et al., 1995; Grant, 1997; Degenhardt et al., 2007; Elgar et al., 2011; Currie and Wild, 2012; Leatherdale and Burkhalter, 2012; Swendsen et al., 2012). Starting drug use during adolescence increases the risk of abuse across the lifetime, with greater vulnerability conferred to younger initiates (Robins and Przybeck, 1985; Grant and Dawson, 1998; Compton et al., 2007; Hasin et al., 2007; McCabe et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2009; Palmer et al., 2009; Swendsen et al., 2012). Significant efforts are being made to develop prevention and early intervention/treatment programs. However, the more we learn about the molecular processes that occur in the adolescent brain and about how these may be influenced by drugs of abuse, the more effective such programs will be.As mentioned above, innervation of the PFC by dopaminergic neurons from the VTA occurs during adolescence. Reynolds et al. (2017) recently showed that, in rodents, delayed development of the mesocortical DA innervation does not result from sprouting of DA fibers that reach this region early in life. Instead, they demonstrated that DA axons continue to grow from the NAc to the PFC during adolescence. Indeed, this is the only known case of long-distance axon growth during this late postnatal age. Netrin-1 guidance cue receptor, DCC, controls this growth by promoting axon targeting events in the NAc, in turn delimiting the number of axons that continue to extend to the cortex. Alterations in DCC expression in DA neurons in adolescence changes the number of axons that continue to extend from the NAc to the PFC and consequently the organization of DA synaptic networks (Manitt et al., 2011, 2013; Reynolds et al., 2017). Importantly, by orchestrating DA connectivity in adolescence, DCC receptors determine the intrinsic structural and functional maturation of the PFC itself (Manitt et al., 2011, 2013; Reynolds et al., 2017). These findings indicate that (1) the path finding trajectory of DA axons remains vulnerable throughout adolescence, and (2) environmental factors that alter DCC receptor expression in DA neurons in adolescence could profoundly influence PFC development. Indeed, exposure to amphetamine in early adolescence at doses that are equivalent to those abused in humans downregulates DCC receptor expression in DA neurons. In turn, this same amphetamine regimen leads to a significant increase in the extent of the DA projections to the PFC but depletes mesocortical axons of presynaptic sites (Reynolds et al., 2015). These enduring effects are not observed following adult exposure to amphetamine, indicating that amphetamine in adolescence disrupts mesocortical DA development via the DCC pathway. Consistent with the determinant role that the mesocortical DA innervation plays on cognitive maturation, we find that amphetamine in adolescence negatively influences cognitive processing in adulthood (Reynolds et al., 2015). Mice exposed to a similar amphetamine treatment in adolescence, but not in adulthood, show exaggerated salience attribution to stimuli paired with the drug and deficits in behavioral inhibition. Age of exposure-dependent effects of amphetamine on cognition has also been seen in rats (Hankosky et al., 2013; Sherrill et al., 2013) and may be sex-dependent (Hammerslag et al., 2014). These changes in salience attribution and cognitive inhibition resemble those observed across psychiatric conditions of PFC dysfunction, including drug abuse.

Development of social reward during adolescence

The adolescent transition from childhood to adulthood requires a qualitative shift in the perception of rewarding social interactions (Spear, 2000). In humans, adolescence is characterized by increases in time spent with peers and changes in the quality of social interactions with family and peers (Larson et al., 1996). Adolescents rely on their contemporaries for social support and are increasingly reactive to treatment by their peers (Ladd et al., 2014). These social relationships influence the development and maintenance of maladaptive behaviors in adulthood (Patterson et al., 1992; Hankin et al., 1998). Indeed, peer influence is a strong predictor of adolescent depression (Thapar et al., 2012). This reorganization of social structure during adolescence is necessary for social species to develop appropriate behavioral strategies for survival in adulthood (Gopnik et al., 2017). A close association between adolescent social reorganization and puberty is thought to increase exposure to genetically distinct individuals when sexual behavior emerges, thereby decreasing the chance of inbreeding within a social group (Lawson Handley and Perrin, 2007). As in humans, adolescent changes in social interactions and social structure are prevalent in rodents. Adolescent rats place a greater value on peer-directed activities (Pellis and Pellis, 2017) and exhibit a greater preference for social stimuli in a conditioned place preference (CPP) test compared with adults (Douglas et al., 2004; Yates et al., 2013), suggesting that the adolescent rodent brain is highly sensitive to social reward. It is thought that adolescent-specific social experiences result in permanent neural and hormonal changes that coalesce in cognitive strategies, which lead to effective coping in adulthood (Spear, 2000). The limbic system is a known regulator of social interaction and social reward. In particular, the amygdala is critically important for the integration of emotional stimuli and regulates emotional and motivated behaviors (Wassum and Izquierdo, 2015). The BLA in particular has been studied extensively for its role in reward because it is thought to be important in assessing/assigning value to stimuli and is a key regulator of social interactions. Activation of the BLA reduces social interaction (Sanders and Shekhar, 1995), whereas inhibition of glutamatergic or GABAergic transmission within the BLA increases social interactions (Sajdyk and Shekhar, 1997; Paine et al., 2017). Projections from the BLA to PFC likely contribute to this social regulation as selective activation of these projections decreases social behaviors (Felix-Ortiz et al., 2016). In addition to its reciprocal glutamatergic projections with the PFC, the BLA projects to the NAc and receives dopaminergic projections from the VTA (Wassum and Izquierdo, 2015). Each of these circuits develops at different stages (Bouwmeester et al., 2002a, b; Cunningham et al., 2002; Caballero et al., 2014; Wassum and Izquierdo, 2015; Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2017). For example, projections from the PFC to BLA are established between P10 and P15 (Bouwmeester et al., 2002b; Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2017), but the reciprocal projections (BLA to PFC) are established a few days earlier (Bouwmeester et al., 2002a; Cunningham et al., 2002). The amygdalar circuit (including NAc, VTA, PFC, and ventral hippocampus) develops during the juvenile/early adolescent period, and synapses are established by the second or third postnatal week. Although projections within the amygdalar circuitry are established before adolescence, recent evidence suggests that the PFC to BLA projections undergo significant synaptic strengthening (as measured by IPSC/EPSC ratio) on P30 (Arruda-Carvalho et al., 2017), and there are more PFC to BLA projections on P31 compared with P24 and P45 (Pattwell et al., 2016), suggesting that this point in adolescence may be a crucial developmental period for limbic structures. The amygdala (Meaney et al., 1981; Meaney and McEwen, 1986; Jessen et al., 2010), and BLA in particular (Trezza et al., 2012; Achterberg et al., 2015), are important regulators of social play, a prominent juvenile social behavior that is important for social, emotional, and cognitive development (Pellegrini, 1988; Vanderschuren et al., 1997; van den Berg et al., 1999; Baarendse et al., 2013). Notably, the PFC-BLA synaptic development coincides with the developmental rise in this behavior (Panksepp, 1981).

Gonadal hormonal influences on adolescent reward valiance

Social interactions in adolescence are not limited to same sex conspecifics. As juveniles proceed through adolescence, interactions with opposite sex conspecifics, and ultimately copulation, become motivating and rewarding stimuli. The performance of sexual behavior may increase risk of aggressive and predatory encounter (Kavaliers and Choleris, 2001), but is also a strong natural reward (Trezza et al., 2011). Thus, it is possible that the neural developmental processes that promote adaptive rewarding social interactions also contribute to risk taking behavior that increases during human adolescence. As mentioned above, adolescent rodents display a greater preference for social stimuli in a CPP paradigm (Douglas et al., 2004; Yates et al., 2013). However, attempts to compare reactions of juvenile and adult animals to potentially rewarding social interactions can be confounded by the conspecific stimulus animal behaving differently toward juveniles and adults. This concern is alleviated by using a social cue, such as opposite sex odors, which can be presented in place of the behaving stimulus animal. The male Syrian hamster provides a unique model for understanding how the perception of potentially rewarding social cues matures across adolescence because their attraction to female vaginal secretions (VSs), which are essential for adult sexual behavior, develops across adolescence (Murphy and Schneider, 1970; Johnston, 1986).VSs are an unconditioned reward to sex-naive adult male hamsters in a CPP test (Bell et al., 2010). Importantly, this same chemosensory cue is not attractive (Johnston and Coplin, 1979) or rewarding (Bell et al., 2013b) to juvenile animals. Like the performance of sex behavior, the rewarding perception of VSs is dependent on circulating testosterone: adult animals that were gonadectomized 10 weeks prior did not show CPP to VSs (Bell and Sisk, 2013). This suggests that the increase in gonadal hormones during puberty normally promotes this adolescent shift in cue interpretation. Indeed, treating juvenile Syrian hamsters with testosterone induces CPP to VS weeks earlier than in unmanipulated hamsters (Bell et al., 2013a), indicating that the shift in VS reward valence is indeed puberty-dependent. The neural circuitry for this shift in behavior includes many of the same brain regions and neurotransmitters involved in drugs of abuse. The display of CPP for VSs in testosterone-treated juvenile animals is blocked by systemic haloperidol, a DA receptor antagonist (Bell and Sisk, 2013). VSs induce expression of the immediate early gene Fos throughout hypothalamic and amygdalar brain regions normally implicated in sociosexual behavior and chemosensory cues equally between juvenile and adult Syrian hamsters (Romeo et al., 1998). However, adult, but not juvenile, males show Fos responses to VS in subregions of the PFC and dopaminergic and nondopaminergic cells in the VTA (Bell et al., 2013b). These immature Fos responses are not transformed into adult-like patterns by testosterone treatment of juvenile males (Bell et al., 2013a). In parallel, testosterone treatment does not induce sex behavior in juvenile animals (Schulz et al., 2009). Perhaps puberty-independent maturation of the mesocorticolimbic system is necessary for the full complement of sociosexual behavior. Complex behaviors are mediated by a suite of brain regions, which may be differentially hormone sensitive. As such, major changes in behavior are likely affected by both puberty-dependent and puberty-independent mechanisms.

Adolescent social stress affects adult reward-associated behaviors and brain regions

Adversity during the adolescent period increases the risk for numerous psychiatric disorders in adulthood, including depression (Heim et al., 2008), anxiety (Espejo et al., 2007), substance use disorders (Scheller-Gilkey et al., 2003), and schizophrenia (Holtzman et al., 2013). Manipulation of the social experience in adolescent rodents results in long-term alterations in the behavioral correlates of anxiety, depression, and substance use disorders (Burke et al., 2017), thereby providing a valuable model in which to investigate the cellular and molecular underpinnings of susceptibility to psychiatric disorders. Specifically, adolescent social isolation stress (aSI) is commonly used to induce susceptibility to substance use disorders as well as anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in adult rodents (Burke et al., 2017). Given its prevalence as a model, it would be beyond the scope of this review to provide a comprehensive overview of the effects of aSI on reward-associated development. However, this manipulation consistently enhances self-administration and CPP for numerous drugs of abuse and natural reward in males (Smith et al., 1997; Howes et al., 2000; Brenes and Fornaguera, 2008; Whitaker et al., 2013), alters social and emotional processing (Einon and Morgan, 1977; Arakawa, 2003; Lukkes et al., 2009b), and increases fear- and anxiety-related behaviors (Einon and Morgan, 1977; Wright et al., 1991a; Wilkinson et al., 1994; Lukkes et al., 2009a; Chappell et al., 2013) in adult rodents. In addition, aSI is associated with changes in gene expression and protein throughout the mesocorticolimbic system (Lukkes et al., 2009b, 2013; Donner et al., 2012; Hickey et al., 2012; Whitaker et al., 2013), in neuronal development and monoaminergic activity in the NAc, BLA, and PFC (Wright et al., 1991b; Fulford and Marsden, 1998a, b; Brenes and Fornaguera, 2008; Brenes et al., 2008; Lukkes et al., 2008, 2009c, 2013; Fabricius et al., 2011) and in electrophysiological properties within the VTA, hippocampus (HIP), and PFC (Peters and O'Donnell, 2005; Ashby et al., 2010; Whitaker et al., 2013). Although it is clear that aSI has profound effects on mesocorticolimbic development and function, to our knowledge, whether these effects are dependent on the pubertal hormone surge and how aSI might alter hormonal influences of mesocorticolimbic processing have not been investigated. Shipping stress and immune challenge during puberty blunt the antidepressant and anxiolytic effects of estradiol and alter microglia activation in the amygdala (AMY) and HIP of adult female rats (Ismail and Blaustein, 2013; Ismail et al., 2013; Holder and Blaustein, 2017). This research offers intriguing evidence that at least nonsocial stressors during adolescence can alter hormonal influences on affective behaviors and neural circuitry in adulthood.Much of the work investigating aSI on reward-associated behaviors has focused on male rodents (Cooke et al., 2000) and the traditional reward-associated brain regions, including the mPFC, NAc, VTA, and BLA (Lukkes et al., 2009b). Nevertheless, females are sensitive to aSI, and the mechanisms underlying their sensitivity are beginning to be investigated (Burke et al., 2017). Initial studies suggest that the effect of aSI on reward-associated behaviors in females is opposite from those in males. As predicted, male mice isolated from P22-P42 display a greater preference for cocaine in a CPP paradigm compared with nonisolates. Females, however, show a reduced preference. aSI also reversed the sex difference in cocaine preference (females > males under control conditions) and markedly reduced or reversed known sex differences in baseline anxiety-like behaviors in adulthood (D. M. Walker and E. J. Nestler, unpublished data). Current research is investigating circuitwide transcriptomic and projection-specific changes altered by aSI to understand how the mesocorticolimbic circuitry is “organized” during adolescence in a sex-specific manner. Brain areas outside the canonical reward-associated circuitry are also likely to play a role in sex differences in aSI, particularly those that are hormone sensitive, sexually dimorphic, and send projections to brain areas of the mesocorticolimbic pathway. The medial AMY (meAMY) is larger in males than in females (Hines et al., 1992; Kerchner et al., 1995). However, unlike most sexually dimorphic brain regions, sex differences in volume of subnuclei within the meAMY do not emerge until adolescence, and the pubertal testosterone surge in males contributes to the organization of this sex difference (De Lorme et al., 2012). This change in meAMY structure co-occurs with changes in rewarding sociosexual behaviors that are in part regulated by it (De Lorme et al., 2012). However, little is known about the role of the meAMY in drug-related reward (Knapska et al., 2007); and to our knowledge, only one study has investigated its role in cocaine self-administration (Kuzmin and Johansson, 1999). Additionally, adolescent stress dysmasculinizes the meAMY: meAMY volume and cell number are decreased in males stressed during adolescence compared with their control counterparts, and these stressed males are less efficient at mating (Cooke et al., 2000). Collectively, this literature suggests that the meAMY contributes to the development, initiation, and maintenance of sex differences in reward and motivation. Consistent with this hypothesis, initial experiments have found that aSI not only reversed known sexually dimorphic behaviors but also reversed sex differences in gene expression throughout the reward circuitry with the strongest effects observed in the meAMY. Furthermore, the sex difference in the number of projections from the meAMY to the VTA is lost after aSI (Walker and Nestler, unpublished data), suggesting that the emergence of many sex differences in meAMY during adolescence are affected by social cues and could be crucial for the manifestation of sex differences in motivation and reward in adulthood.

Exploiting seasonal physiology to separate puberty-dependent and puberty-independent influences on adolescent reward

As discussed in this review, the development of reward-associated behaviors and circuitry is influenced by both puberty-dependent and puberty-independent mechanisms. Disentangling contributions from puberty-dependent and puberty-independent processes, however, is often difficult. Traditional approaches to this question fall under two categories. The first asks whether gonadal hormone manipulations alter the timing of adolescent development: Does gonadectomy prevent, and hormone replacement restore, adolescent changes? The second approach asks whether adolescent changes correlate with chronological age or pubertal status. This second approach is often difficult because, in most models, puberty is tightly correlated with chronological age leading to small age differences among conspecifics in the timing of pubertal development. Seasonally breeding species could provide an alternate approach to investigate puberty-dependent and puberty-independent influences on adolescent development; one that uses natural adaptations to dissociate pubertal development from chronological age in structurally and genetically intact animals. For many seasonally breeding rodents, offspring born early in the breeding season (spring and early summer) undergo rapid pubertal development to breed that same year, whereas those born at the end of the breeding season (autumn) delay puberty during the winter months to synchronize the onset of breeding with the following spring (Sadleir, 1969). Day length serves as a proximate cue for time of year in many seasonally breeding species (Paul et al., 2008; Stevenson et al., 2017). Hence, seasonal regulation of pubertal development can be recapitulated in the laboratory simply by manipulating the ambient photoperiod: long, summer-like day lengths (LDs) stimulate rapid pubertal development, whereas short, winter-like day lengths (SDs) delay pubertal development (Whitsett and Miller, 1982; Yellon and Goldman, 1984; Edmonds and Stetson, 1993; Nelson et al., 1997). In Siberian hamsters (Phodopus sungorus), SD rearing delays puberty by 3–5 months (Hoffmann, 1978) (Fig. 2A). Hence, in seasonal species, one can systematically control the timing of puberty with photoperiod and ask whether the development of nonreproductive adolescent traits is similarly delayed by SD rearing (puberty-dependent; Fig. 2B) or unaffected by ambient photoperiod (puberty-independent; Fig. 2C).

Figure 2. Using seasonal breeders to separate puberty-dependent and puberty-independent influences on adolescent development. A, Photoperiod regulation of puberty in male Siberian hamsters: LDs stimulate, whereas SDs delay, testicular development. B, C, How this model can be used to determine whether the development of an adolescent trait is regulated by puberty-dependent or puberty-independent mechanisms. Solid blue line indicates the developmental profile of a hypothetical trait for LD-reared hamsters. Dashed green line indicates the developmental profile of a hypothetical trait for SD-reared hamsters. Delayed development of the adolescent trait in SD-reared hamsters (as in B) maintains temporal synchrony with puberty and indicates puberty-dependent regulation. Absence of a photoperiod effect on the adolescent trait (as in C), however, dissociates the trait from puberty in SD-reared hamsters and indicates puberty-independent regulation. ETV, Estimated testis volume. Illustration of testis data modified with permission from Paul et al. (2006).It should be noted that the seasonal species approach asks a slightly different question than the gonadal hormone approach. Manipulating gonadal hormones during adolescence directly tests the role of increased gonadal hormones at puberty, whereas shifting the timing of puberty with photoperiod tests the role of pubertal activation of the entire HPG axis. Indeed, gonadectomy does not prevent pubertal onset: activation of the hypothalamic GnRH pulse generator and increased secretion of pituitary gonadotropins at puberty occur even in the absence of the gonads (Ojeda and Terasawa, 2002). The seasonal species approach circumvents potential confounds of the gonadal hormone approach, including early-life surgical/handling stress (Vetter-O'Hagen and Spear, 2011; Pritchard et al., 2012; Vetter-O'Hagen and Spear, 2012b; Farrell et al., 2016) and compensatory changes in the brain and pituitary after removal of gonadal steroid feedback (Meijs-Roelofs and Kramer, 1979; Andrews and Ojeda, 1981; Bittman et al., 1996; Dubois et al., 2016). On the other hand, photoperiod can influence more than just puberty in seasonal species (Paul et al., 2008; Stevenson et al., 2017), and contributions from other seasonal traits must be considered if there is an effect of photoperiod on adolescent development (as in Fig. 2B). This is not an issue if photoperiod does not influence the development of the adolescent trait (as in Fig. 2C). Hence, the two approaches complement each other and are best used in combination. Experiments using Siberian hamsters have begun to test the seasonal species approach on the development of two reward-associated behaviors: one thought to be regulated by puberty-independent mechanisms: social play (Smith et al., 1996); and another for which the data are mixed: novelty seeking (Cyrenne and Brown, 2011; Vetter-O'Hagen and Spear, 2012a). Juvenile rats can be conditioned to prefer a location in which they previously played (Calcagnetti and Schechter, 1992; Trezza et al., 2009) and will work for access to a playmate (Achterberg et al., 2016). Hence, it is not surprising that social play is regulated by neurotransmitter systems implicated in reward, including the mesolimbic DA pathway (Trezza et al., 2010; Achterberg et al., 2016; Manduca et al., 2016). Novelty seeking is also regulated by reward-related circuits and mesolimbic DA (Hooks and Kalivas, 1995; Rebec et al., 1997; Wingo et al., 2016). Both social play and novelty seeking peak in juveniles/early adolescents and then decline thereafter (Panksepp, 1981; Pellis and Pellis, 1990; Douglas et al., 2003; Stansfield and Kirstein, 2006; Vetter-O'Hagen and Spear, 2012a). Siberian hamsters also exhibit the same periadolescent declines in social play and novelty seeking when reared in an LD photoperiod that triggers rapid pubertal development (M. J. Paul, C. K. Probst, L. M. Brown, and G. J. de Vries, unpublished data; R. F. Kyne, Q. E. Carroll, K. C. Schatz, R. Y. Bivieca, and M. J. Paul, unpublished data). When puberty is delayed by SD rearing, the decline in play is completed before pubertal onset and the development of play is dissociated from puberty. The decline in novelty seeking, however, is delayed in SD-reared Siberian hamsters and remains coincident with puberty. These data suggest that the development of play is puberty-independent, whereas that of novelty seeking is puberty-dependent. While further research is necessary, this model provides a novel and exciting approach to disentangle puberty-dependent and puberty-independent influences on adolescent behavioral and neural development.

Emerging themes

The factors contributing to adolescent reward are many, and we are only beginning to understand the complex interactions between neural networks, endocrine axes, and environmental cues that direct the development of a functioning male- and female-typical mesocorticolimbic reward circuit. The many behavioral changes and neuroendocrine interactions may seem chaotic, but it is clear that adolescent development is a highly regulated and coordinated process. In this review, we have highlighted a few overarching themes that are beginning to emerge from the chaos: (1) There are notable sex differences in adolescent development that might underlie sexually dimorphic reward-associated behaviors in adulthood. (2) The mesocorticolimbic pathway is critical for adolescent changes in social reward and reward learning. (3) Reorganization of the reward circuitry, particularly the PFC, during adolescence relies on social interactions, pubertal hormones, as well as nonpubertal processes. (4) Adolescent reward circuitry is highly vulnerable to social stress and drugs of abuse. Further research is necessary for a comprehensive understanding of the factors that regulate development of the mesocorticolimbic pathway, those that lead to increased vulnerability to disruption, and how this process drives developmental changes in motivation and reward. This research would benefit from the use of multiple approaches and models to disentangle the neural, endocrine, and environmental influences on adolescent reward. Together, these investigations will provide valuable insight into sex-specific psychiatric and behavioral disorders that arise during adolescence and could lead to novel avenues for treatment and prevention.

Link to Article

Abstract

During the teen years and young adulthood, people develop more social, emotional, and thinking skills, but also take more risks, like trying drugs. This happens in part because young people are more sensitive to rewards and have less self-control. Difficulty during this time of life can lead to long-lasting changes in how the brain continues to change into adulthood. This review discusses how connections in the brain, hormones during puberty, social situations, and the environment affect how young people learn to respond to rewards.

Adolescent Reward: Interactions between Brain, Hormones, and Environment

Introduction

Adolescence is a transformative period characterized by heightened reward sensitivity and vulnerability to stress and psychiatric disorders. This vulnerability is partly due to the ongoing development of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine (DA) system, which plays a crucial role in reward processing. Pubertal hormones, environmental perturbations (e.g., drug use and social stress), and puberty-independent factors interact to shape the development of this system.

Sex Differences in Reward

Adolescent reward exhibits sex differences. Males tend to exhibit greater reward sensitivity and risk-taking than females. These differences are reflected in brain structure and function, with males showing delayed development of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and larger striatal volume.

Development of the PFC

The PFC, a key regulator of reward-directed behaviors, undergoes significant development during adolescence. DA projections to the PFC mature later than those to other brain regions. This protracted development contributes to the cognitive and behavioral changes observed during adolescence.

Pubertal Influences on PFC Development

Puberty influences PFC development. In rats, pubertal onset leads to neuronal loss and synaptic pruning in the PFC, particularly in females. These changes are associated with improved cognitive flexibility.

Disruption of PFC Development by Drugs of Abuse

Drugs of abuse can disrupt PFC development during adolescence. Amphetamine exposure in early adolescence alters the trajectory of DA axon growth, leading to increased DA projections to the PFC and deficits in cognitive function.

Development of Social Reward

Adolescence is a time of increased social interaction and sensitivity to social rewards. The amygdala, particularly the basolateral amygdala (BLA), plays a crucial role in social reward. Projections from the PFC to the BLA develop during adolescence and contribute to the regulation of social behaviors.

Gonadal Hormonal Influences on Social Reward

Puberty-dependent increases in gonadal hormones promote the development of social reward. In Syrian hamsters, testosterone induces a shift in the perception of female vaginal secretions from aversive to rewarding. This shift is associated with increased Fos expression in the PFC and DAergic regions of the brain.

Adolescent Social Stress and Reward

Social stress during adolescence can have long-term effects on reward-associated behaviors and brain regions. Adolescent social isolation stress (aSI) enhances drug and natural reward seeking, alters gene expression and neuronal development in the mesocorticolimbic system, and increases anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in adulthood. Sex differences have been observed in the effects of aSI, with females showing reduced reward preference and altered brain circuitry.

Exploiting Seasonal Physiology to Disentangle Puberty-Dependent and -Independent Influences

Seasonally breeding species provide a unique model to investigate the relative contributions of puberty-dependent and -independent processes to adolescent development. By manipulating photoperiod to control the timing of puberty, researchers can determine whether adolescent traits develop in synchrony with puberty (puberty-dependent) or independently of it (puberty-independent).

Emerging Themes

  • Sex differences in adolescent reward are influenced by brain structure, function, and hormonal factors.

  • The mesocorticolimbic pathway undergoes significant development during adolescence, contributing to changes in social reward and reward learning.

  • Pubertal hormones, social interactions, and puberty-independent factors interact to shape the development of the reward circuitry.

  • Adolescent reward circuitry is vulnerable to disruption by social stress and drugs of abuse, which can have long-term consequences for mental health.

Link to Article

Abstract

During the teen years and young adulthood, people develop more social, emotional, and thinking skills, but also take more risks, like trying drugs. This happens in part because young people are more sensitive to rewards and have less self-control. Difficulty during this time of life can lead to long-lasting changes in how the brain continues to change into adulthood. This review discusses how connections in the brain, hormones during puberty, social situations, and the environment affect how young people learn to respond to rewards.

Adolescent Brain Development and Reward

Introduction

Adolescence is a time of major changes in the brain and body. It's when young people transition from childhood to adulthood, and it's a time of increased exploration and risk-taking. This is partly because the brain's reward system is still developing during adolescence.

Sex Differences in Reward

  • Adolescents are more sensitive to rewards than adults.

  • Boys and girls show different patterns of reward sensitivity.

  • Girls tend to peak in reward sensitivity earlier than boys.

  • Boys show greater reward sensitivity to food and video games.

Development of the Prefrontal Cortex (PFC)

  • The PFC is the part of the brain that helps us control our impulses and make decisions.

  • It continues to develop throughout adolescence.

  • Puberty may play a role in PFC development, especially in girls.

Disruption of PFC Development by Drugs

  • Drugs of abuse, such as amphetamine, can disrupt the development of the PFC.

  • This can lead to problems with decision-making and impulse control.

Social Reward

  • Adolescents place a high value on social interactions.

  • The brain's reward system is activated by social interactions.

  • Social experiences during adolescence can shape the development of the reward system.

Gonadal Hormones and Reward

  • Gonadal hormones (e.g., testosterone and estrogen) play a role in reward processing.

  • Testosterone may increase reward sensitivity to social cues in boys.

Adolescent Social Stress and Reward

  • Social stress during adolescence can alter the development of the reward system.

  • This can increase the risk of developing mental health problems, such as depression and anxiety.

Using Seasonal Physiology to Study Puberty-Dependent and Puberty-Independent Influences

  • Researchers can use seasonal breeders, such as hamsters, to study the effects of puberty on brain development.

  • By manipulating the amount of light hamsters are exposed to, researchers can delay or accelerate puberty.

  • This allows them to separate the effects of puberty from other factors that may influence brain development.

Conclusion

Adolescence is a critical period for the development of the reward system. Puberty, social experiences, and environmental factors all play a role in shaping this system. Understanding these factors can help us better understand and prevent mental health problems that arise during adolescence.

Link to Article

Abstract

During the teen years and young adulthood, people develop more social, emotional, and thinking skills, but also take more risks, like trying drugs. This happens in part because young people are more sensitive to rewards and have less self-control. Difficulty during this time of life can lead to long-lasting changes in how the brain continues to change into adulthood. This review discusses how connections in the brain, hormones during puberty, social situations, and the environment affect how young people learn to respond to rewards.

The Teenage Brain and Rewards: A Time of Change and Vulnerability

What's Happening in the Teenage Brain?

As you enter your teenage years, your brain is going through a period of major development. This is called adolescence, and it's a time when your brain is changing in important ways. One of the key areas that's changing is the mesocorticolimbic pathway, which is your brain's reward system.

The Reward System

Your reward system helps you feel good when you do things that are important for your survival, like eating, drinking, and spending time with friends. It releases a chemical called dopamine, which gives you a feeling of pleasure.

Changes in the Reward System During Adolescence

During adolescence, your reward system becomes more sensitive. This means that you're more likely to feel good when you do rewarding things. It also means that you're more likely to take risks and seek out new experiences.

Sex Differences

Boys and girls experience adolescence differently. For example, boys tend to have a larger increase in dopamine release in their reward system than girls. This may explain why boys are more likely to engage in risky behaviors, such as drug use and reckless driving.

The Importance of the Prefrontal Cortex

The prefrontal cortex (PFC) is the part of your brain that helps you make good decisions and control your impulses. It's still developing during adolescence, which means that teenagers may have a harder time resisting temptations and making responsible choices.

Social Influences

Your social environment plays a big role in how your reward system develops. Spending time with friends and family can help you develop a healthy reward system. On the other hand, negative social experiences, such as bullying or isolation, can disrupt this development.

Vulnerability to Drugs and Stress

The changes in the reward system during adolescence make teenagers more vulnerable to drug use and stress. Drugs of abuse can hijack the reward system and lead to addiction. Stress can also disrupt the development of the reward system and increase the risk of mental health problems.

Conclusion

Adolescence is a time of great change and opportunity. It's also a time when your brain is particularly vulnerable. Understanding the changes that are happening in your reward system can help you make healthy choices and avoid potential risks.

Link to Article

Abstract

During the teen years and young adulthood, people develop more social, emotional, and thinking skills, but also take more risks, like trying drugs. This happens in part because young people are more sensitive to rewards and have less self-control. Difficulty during this time of life can lead to long-lasting changes in how the brain continues to change into adulthood. This review discusses how connections in the brain, hormones during puberty, social situations, and the environment affect how young people learn to respond to rewards.

Growing Up: How the Brain Learns About Rewards

What's Different About Teenagers?

Have you ever noticed that teenagers seem to be more excited about things than adults? They might get really happy about winning a game or really sad about a breakup. That's because their brains are still developing, especially the parts that control how they feel about rewards.

The Reward Center

Your brain has a special area called the "reward center." When you do something you enjoy, like eating ice cream or playing video games, this area releases a chemical called dopamine. Dopamine makes you feel good and makes you want to do those things again.

Teenagers and Dopamine

Teenagers have more dopamine in their reward centers than adults. This means they feel more excited and happy when they get rewards. It also means they're more likely to take risks to get those rewards.

Changes in the Brain

During the teenage years, the brain is still growing and changing. The part of the brain that helps you make good decisions, called the prefrontal cortex, is still developing. This means that teenagers might not always think about the consequences of their actions before they do them.

Social Rewards

As teenagers get older, they start to care more about social rewards, like being popular or having friends. This is because the parts of their brain that control social behavior are also developing.

Hormones

Hormones, which are chemicals that control your body's growth and development, also play a role in how teenagers feel about rewards. When teenagers go through puberty, their bodies start to produce more hormones like testosterone and estrogen. These hormones can make teenagers more sensitive to rewards.

Stress and Drugs

Unfortunately, some things can mess up the way the reward center develops. Stress, like being bullied or having a lot of homework, can make teenagers more likely to use drugs or alcohol. Drugs and alcohol can also damage the developing brain and make it harder for teenagers to control their behavior.

Learning from Experience

The good news is that the teenage brain is very good at learning from experience. This means that teenagers can learn to make good decisions and avoid risky behaviors. By understanding how the reward center works, we can help teenagers make healthy choices and grow into happy and successful adults.

Link to Article

Footnotes and Citation

Cite

Walker, D. M., Bell, M. R., Flores, C., Gulley, J. M., Willing, J., & Paul, M. J. (2017). Adolescence and reward: making sense of neural and behavioral changes amid the chaos. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(45), 10855-10866. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1834-17.2017

    Highlights